Elsevier

Neuropsychologia

Volume 53, January 2014, Pages 284-292
Neuropsychologia

Distinct associations of insula and cingulate volume with the cognitive and affective dimensions of alexithymia

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.12.006Get rights and content

Highlights

  • The two alexithymia dimensions are associated with distinct structural correlates.

  • Right insula volume is larger in high-scorers on the cognitive dimension.

  • Right cingulate volume is larger in individuals with low emotional reactivity.

  • Alexithymia should be regarded as a two-dimensional personality construct.

Abstract

Alexithymia (“no words for feelings”) is a major risk factor for psychosomatic and psychiatric conditions characterized by affect dysregulation. The alexithymia personality construct comprises an affective dimension, the level of subjective emotional experience (emotionalizing and fantasizing), and a cognitive dimension, referring to the cognitive control of emotions (identifying, analyzing, and verbalizing feelings). These two dimensions may differentially put individuals at risk for psychopathology, but their specific neural bases have rarely been investigated. Therefore, the aim of the present study was to find out whether the two alexithymia dimensions are associated with discriminable neural correlates. By means of voxel-based morphometry (VBM), differences in gray matter volumes were compared between 20 (10 male) high-scorers and 20 (9 male) low-scorers on the Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20), reflecting the cognitive alexithymia dimension. In a subset of 32 subjects, the impact of the affective alexithymia dimension was tested in addition, as assessed with the affective subscale of the Bermond–Vorst Alexithymia Questionnaire (BVAQ). Analysis 1 (cognitive alexithymia dimension) revealed significantly larger gray matter volumes in the right posterior insula in high-scorers compared to low-scorers on the TAS-20. Analysis 2 (affective alexithymia dimension) revealed that the affective alexithymia dimension, specifically the emotionalizing factor indicative of low emotional reactivity, was associated with larger gray matter volumes of the right cingulate cortex. These results suggest that the two alexithymia dimensions are associated with distinct structural correlates.

Introduction

With a prevalence rate of ten percent in the general population, alexithymia (“no words for feelings”) is a major risk factor for a range of medical and psychiatric disorders (Taylor, Bagby, & Parker, 1997), including somatoform (Waller & Scheidt, 2004) and panic disorders (Parker, Taylor, & Bagby, 1993). In general, men seem to exhibit higher levels of alexithymia than women, though gender differences are small (Levant, Hall, Williams, & Hasan, 2009). Although alexithymia has long been thought of as a unidimensional construct, it is now acknowledged that it comprises two dimensions, an affective and a cognitive one (Vorst & Bermond, 2001). The cognitive dimension refers to the processing of emotions at the cognitive level and comprises low abilities to identify, analyze, and verbalize one's feelings. These three cognitive alexithymia facets are traditionally assessed with the TAS-20 Toronto Alexithymia Scale (Bagby, Parker, & Taylor, 1994a; Bagby, Taylor, & Parker, 1994b), which comprises the three subscales ‘difficulty identifying feelings', ‘difficulty describing feelings', and ‘externally oriented thinking'. For the TAS-20, a clinical cut-off score has been established that classifies a score equal to or higher than 61 as a clinically relevant alexithymia score (Taylor et al., 1997). The affective alexithymia dimension refers to the level of subjective emotional experience and comprises low degrees of emotional arousal in response to emotion-inducing events (emotionalizing factor), and reduced imaginative capabilities (fantasizing factor). While these affective factors are not part of the TAS-20, they can be assessed by means of the Bermond–Vorst Alexithymia Questionnaire (BVAQ, Vorst & Bermond, 2001).

Based on these two dimensions, different subtypes of alexithymia have been proposed (Bermond et al., 2007). Individuals with type 1 alexithymia are characterized by high scores on both alexithymia dimensions (i.e., the cognitive processing of emotions as well as the level of subjective emotional experience is reduced) and may be perceived as ‘cold-blooded’ personalities as they experience little emotional arousal. This type has been proposed to underlie schizoid personality (Moormann et al., 2008) and psychopathy, in which physiological reactions to emotional stimuli are low (Levenston, Patrick, Bradley, & Lang, 2000) and the cognitive processing of emotions is impaired (Lander, Lutz-Zois, Rye, & Goodnight, 2011). In contrast, individuals with type 2 alexithymia may experience emotional arousal to a normal or even heightened extent, but have difficulty regulating their feelings at the cognitive level (i.e., the cognitive processing of emotions is reduced while the subjective experience of emotions is unaffected). Individuals with type 2 alexithymia tend to be emotionally labile as seen in patients with borderline personality disorder (Moormann et al., 2008). This subtype has also been linked to schizophrenia (van der Meer, van’t Wout, & Aleman, 2009), which is in line with the theory of the emotional paradox stating that these patients do experience emotions but are unable to show them (Aleman & Kahn, 2005). Taken together, type 1 and type 2 alexithymia might differentially put individuals at risk for psychopathology. However, the neural correlates of the cognitive and affective dimensions underlying the alexithymia subtypes are still poorly understood.

Three brain regions seem to be particularly implicated in emotion processing deficits associated with alexithymia: the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), the amygdala, and the insula. The ACC is involved in the awareness and monitoring of one’s emotional experiences (Heinzel et al., 2010a, Medford and Critchley, 2010) and has been proposed to be a key region in alexithymia (Lane, Ahern, Schwartz, & Kaszniak, 1997). Given its involvement in both emotional experience and cognitively demanding emotional tasks, the ACC could be implicated in both alexithymia dimensions (Bermond et al., 2006, Lane et al., 1997; Larsen, Brand, Bermond, & Hijman, 2003; Wingbermühle, Theunissen, Verhoeven, Kessels, & Egger, 2012). The amygdala is an essential structure for the processing of emotions as it is involved in the evaluation of emotional significance, fear conditioning, emotional reactivity, and general salience detection (Adolphs, 2008, Adolphs, 2010, Sergerie et al., 2008). The insula takes part in the cognitive processing of emotions as well as in the generation of emotional states (Medford and Critchley, 2010, Phillips et al., 2003), and is a key region in the subjective experience of feelings derived from bodily states and emotional arousal (Critchley et al., 2004, Diekhof et al., 2011). In addition, the insula regulates autonomic activity in reaction to salient stimuli (Menon & Uddin, 2010), is directly involved in pain perception (Craig et al., 2000, Ostrowsky et al., 2002), and lesions to the insula may cause deficits in balance, light touch, proprioception, pain, taste (Cereda, Ghika, Maeder, & Bogousslavsky, 2002), and heartbeat perception (Khalsa, Rudrauf, Feinstein, & Tranel, 2009). The amygdala and the insula are thought to relate to both alexithymia dimensions, given their involvement in empathy, emotionalizing as well as analyzing emotions (Wingbermühle et al., 2012). Functional imaging studies have reported altered functioning of the ACC (Berthoz et al., 2002, Frewen et al., 2008, Heinzel et al., 2010a, Huber et al., 2002, Moriguchi et al., 2007), the amygdala (Goerlich-Dobre et al., 2013b, Kugel et al., 2008, Miyake et al., 2012, Reker et al., 2010, Zotev et al., 2011) as well as the insula (Frewen et al., 2006, Heinzel et al., 2010b, Kano et al., 2003, Kano et al., 2007, Moriguchi et al., 2007, Reker et al., 2010, Silani et al., 2008) in relation to alexithymia (for a meta-analysis, see van der Velde et al., 2013). However, all these studies used the TAS-20 scale for alexithymia assessment, which assesses only the cognitive alexithymia dimension. To date, only one functional imaging study took the affective alexithymia dimension into account and found that low emotional reactivity was linked to hyperactivity of the dorsal ACC in response to fearful faces (Pouga, Berthoz, de Gelder, & Grezes, 2010).

Supplementing functional studies, structural imaging studies have begun to reveal changes in cerebral morphology associated with the cognitive alexithymia dimension. Initial studies using manual tracing techniques focused on the ACC as a region of interest (ROI), based on Lane’s hypothesis of a core deficit in emotional self-awareness in alexithymia. The first study of this kind found a positive correlation between the cognitive alexithymia dimension and the surface area of the right ACC (Gündel et al., 2004), whereas a second study reported a negative correlation between alexithymia and gray matter volumes of the right rostral ACC (Paradiso, Vaidya, McCormick, Jones, & Robinson, 2008). Subsequent studies using automated Voxel-Based Morphometry (VBM) methods reported reduced ACC volumes (Borsci et al., 2009, Ihme et al., 2013, Sturm and Levenson, 2011) or no volume differences of the ACC in relation to the cognitive alexithymia dimension (Heinzel et al., 2012, Kubota et al., 2011, Zhang et al., 2011). Besides the ACC, structural differences of the insula were observed in some studies (Borsci et al., 2009, Ihme et al., 2013, Zhang et al., 2011), but not in others (Heinzel et al., 2012, Kubota et al., 2011; Sturm & Levenson, 2011). Regarding the amygdala, one study reported smaller amygdala volumes in high-scorers on alexithymia compared to gender-matched low-scorers (Ihme et al., 2013). In addition to the ACC, insula, and amygdala, structural differences in other regions have sporadically been found in relation to the cognitive alexithymia dimension, including middle temporal gyrus (Borsci et al., 2009, Ihme et al., 2013), superior temporal sulcus (Borsci et al., 2009), orbitofrontal gyrus (Borsci et al., 2009), ventral striatum, ventral premotor cortex, and supramarginal gyrus (Kubota et al., 2011). All of these previous studies used the TAS-20 for the assessment of alexithymia, and therefore previous findings can only be related to the cognitive alexithymia dimension, while the impact of the affective alexithymia dimension on morphometric differences has not been investigated yet.

Taken together, results of previous structural studies are equivocal, and no clear picture has yet emerged regarding the morphological underpinnings and the directionality of differences in gray matter volume in relation to alexithymia. Conclusions with respect to gray matter volume differences are further complicated by the fact that such differences may be interpreted in the light of recruitment of additional (compensatory) processing resources, or in the light of a positive relationship between the regular use of a brain region and its size (Maguire et al., 2000). Discrepancies between previous structural alexithymia studies may be due to variance in the range of alexithymia scores between studies (below or above the clinical threshold score on the TAS-20), differences in analysis strategies (region of interest versus whole brain approach), and finally, the fact that all previous structural studies considered only the cognitive alexithymia dimension, despite findings indicating that also the affective alexithymia dimension modulates emotion processing (Bermond et al., 2010, Goerlich et al., 2012, Moormann et al., 2008, Pouga et al., 2010).

The present VBM study investigated the impact of the cognitive(c) alexithymia dimension on gray matter volume in 40 female and male age- and education-matched individuals with either low scores (Low ALEXc group) or scores equal to or higher than the clinical cut-off score on the TAS-20 alexithymia scale (High ALEXc group), in keeping with previous studies. In addition, we tested the impact of the affective alexithymia dimension (i.e., difficulty emotionalizing and fantasizing) on gray matter volumes as assessed with the affective subscales of the BVAQ (Bermond–Vorst Alexithymia Questionnaire) (Vorst & Bermond, 2001) in a subset of 32 subjects. We hypothesized gray matter volume differences in emotion-related brain areas previously suggested to alter in structure and function in alexithymia, particularly the ACC, amygdala, and insula. We further hypothesized that the affective and cognitive alexithymia dimensions would be associated with distinct structural correlates.

Section snippets

Participants

A total of 1128 individuals filled out the TAS-20 questionnaire online through the community-based study pool run by the Department of Psychology at Harvard University (TAS-20 score range 20–80, mean 44.52±11.81). The cut-off score of 61 on the TAS-20 was used as the minimum score for the group of participants scoring high on the cognitive alexithymia dimension (High ALEXc). As there is no established cut-off score for low alexithymia, a TAS-20 score of 35 (i.e., the 25th percentile) was used

Behavioral data

Results of the alexithymia scale ratings and characteristics of the two ALEXc groups are presented in Table 1. Internal consistencies as indexed by Cronbach’s α ranged from acceptable to excellent for the three subscales of the TAS-20 (DIF, α=.912; DDF, α=.800; EOT, α=.703) and was good for the two affective subscales of the BVAQ (emotionalizing, α=.829; fantasizing, α=.829). The three TAS-20 subscales were significantly correlated with each other (DIF and DDF, r=.895, p<0.001; DDF and EOT, r

Discussion

The present study aimed to find out whether the cognitive and affective dimensions of alexithymia are associated with distinct neural substrates in a group of female and male individuals with TAS-20 scores above the clinical threshold, compared to a group of individuals with low alexithymia scores. We found that the cognitive alexithymia dimension was related to significantly larger gray matter volumes of the right posterior insula, whereas the affective dimension was associated with larger

Acknowledgements

We thank Tammy Moran for her invaluable technical support during scanning at the CBS. We further thank Laura Tully, Sarah Hope Lincoln, and Laura Germine for their advice regarding the design of the study, and Chinmayi Tengshe, Velina Sevdalinova, and Taylor Benson for their help in participant recruitment, screening, and scanning. We further thank Remco Renken and Thilo Kellermann for their support during imaging data analysis as well as David Weyer from the Brain Imaging Facility of the

References (88)

  • A. Heinzel et al.

    Alexithymia in healthy young men: A voxel-based morphometric study

    Journal of Affective Disorders

    (2012)
  • K. Ihme et al.

    Alexithymia is related to differences in gray matter volume: A voxel-based morphometry study

    Brain Research

    (2013)
  • M. Kano et al.

    Correlation between alexithymia and hypersensitivity to visceral stimulation in human

    Pain

    (2007)
  • M. Kubota et al.

    Alexithymia and regional gray matter alterations in schizophrenia

    Neuroscience Research

    (2011)
  • S. Kühn et al.

    The neural correlates of subjective pleasantness

    NeuroImage

    (2012)
  • H. Kugel et al.

    Alexithymic features and automatic amygdala reactivity to facial emotion

    Neuroscience Letters

    (2008)
  • R.D. Lane et al.

    Is alexithymia the emotional equivalent of blindsight?

    Biological Psychiatry

    (1997)
  • J.K. Larsen et al.

    Cognitive and emotional characteristics of alexithymia: a review of neurobiological studies

    Journal of Psychosomatic Research

    (2003)
  • E. Lemche et al.

    Interoceptive-reflective regions differentiate alexithymia traits in depersonalization disorder

    Psychiatry Research

    (2013)
  • K. Mériau et al.

    A neural network reflecting individual differences in cognitive processing of emotions during perceptual decision making

    NeuroImage

    (2006)
  • Y. Miyake et al.

    Brain activation during the perception of stressful word stimuli concerning interpersonal relationships in anorexia nervosa patients with high degrees of alexithymia in an fMRI paradigm

    Psychiatry Research

    (2012)
  • S. Paradiso et al.

    Aging and alexithymia: Association with reduced right rostral cingulate volume

    American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry

    (2008)
  • J.D. A. Parker et al.

    The relationship between emotional intelligence and alexithymia

    Personality and Individual Differences

    (2001)
  • M.L. Phillips et al.

    Neurobiology of emotion perception I: The neural basis of normal emotion perception

    Biological Psychiatry

    (2003)
  • M. Reker et al.

    Individual differences in alexithymia and brain response to masked emotion faces

    Cortex

    (2010)
  • K. Sergerie et al.

    The role of the amygdala in emotional processing: A quantitative meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies

    Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews

    (2008)
  • M. Silver et al.

    False positives in neuroimaging genetics using voxel-based morphometry data

    NeuroImage

    (2011)
  • H. Takeuchi et al.

    Regional gray matter volume of dopaminergic system associate with creativity: Evidence from voxel-based morphometry

    NeuroImage

    (2010)
  • L. van der Meer et al.

    Emotion regulation strategies in patients with schizophrenia

    Psychiatry Research

    (2009)
  • J. van der Velde et al.

    Neural correlates of alexithymia: A meta-analysis of emotion processing studies

    Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews

    (2013)
  • H. Vorst et al.

    Validity and reliability of the Bermond–Vorst alexithymia questionnaire

    Personality and Individual Differences

    (2001)
  • E. Waller et al.

    Somatoform disorders as disorders of affect regulation: A study comparing the TAS-20 with non-self-report measures of alexithymia

    Journal of Psychosomatic Research

    (2004)
  • T. White et al.

    Anatomic and functional variability: The effects of filter size in group fMRI data analysis

    NeuroImage

    (2001)
  • X. Zhang et al.

    Factors underlying prefrontal and insula structural alterations in smokers

    NeuroImage

    (2011)
  • R. Adolphs

    What does the amygdala contribute to social cognition?

    Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences

    (2010)
  • R. Adolphs et al.

    Neurobiology of emotion at a systems level

    The Neuropsychology of Emotion

    (2000)
  • M. Beauregard et al.

    Neural correlates of conscious self-regulation of emotion

    Journal of Neuroscience

    (2001)
  • B. Bermond et al.

    A cognitive and an affective dimension of alexithymia in six languages and seven populations

    Cognition and Emotion

    (2007)
  • B. Bermond et al.

    Cognitive neuropsychology of alexithymia: Implications for personality typology

    Cognitive Neuropsychiatry

    (2006)
  • S. Berthoz et al.

    Effect of impaired recognition and expression of emotions on frontocingulate cortices: An fMRI study of men with alexithymia

    American Journal of Psychiatry

    (2002)
  • S. Berthoz et al.

    Comparison of the psychometric properties of two self-report questionnaires measuring alexithymia; confirmatory factor analysis of the 20-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale and the Bermond–Vorst alexithymia questionnaire

    European Review of Applied Psychology

    (2000)
  • G. Bird et al.

    Empathic brain responses in insula are modulated by levels of alexithymia but not autism

    Brain

    (2010)
  • C. Cereda et al.

    Strokes restricted to the insular cortex

    Neurology

    (2002)
  • A.D. Craig et al.

    Thermosensory activation of insular cortex

    Nature Neuroscience

    (2000)
  • Cited by (74)

    • Determinants and impact of alexithymia on quality of life in Parkinson's disease

      2023, Neurologia
      Citation Excerpt :

      From a pathophysiological viewpoint it has been hypothesized neurodegeneration at basal ganglia, vastly connected with the dorsal prefrontal and orbitofrontal areas may explain impairment in emotional experience processing.5,7 Neuroimaging studies in persons with alexithymia have implicated other areas such as the insula and the cingulate cortex.8,9 In addition, alexithymia may cause psychological distress leading to social and emotional dysfunction and psychiatric disorders that might result in a poor QoL, disability and caregiver burden.10

    • Gray matter structural plasticity in patients with basal ganglia germ cell tumors: A voxel-based morphometry study

      2022, Magnetic Resonance Imaging
      Citation Excerpt :

      This study found gray matter increase in the parahippocampal cortex and MCC in patients with BGGCTs. A previous study observed a positive correlation between affective alexithymia scores and MCC volume [35]. The regulation of emotional responses, particularly attempts to suppress arousal, have been repeatedly associated with cingulate activity [36].

    • Atypical interoception as a common risk factor for psychopathology: A review

      2021, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews
      Citation Excerpt :

      Notably, certain items on the TAS-20 are likely to assess interoceptive difficulties (e.g., “I have physical sensations that even doctors don’t understand”, “I am often puzzled by sensations in my body”, “I don’t know what’s going on inside me”). Further support for the association between alexithymia and interoception comes from atypicalities in the structure and function of interoceptive cortex (AI and ACC) in alexithymia, which seem relatively specific to these regions, rather than diffuse across the brain (e.g. Borsci et al., 2009; Deng et al., 2013; Frewen et al., 2006; Goerlich-Dobre et al., 2014; Grabe et al., 2014; Heinzel et al., 2010; Ihme et al., 2013; Jongen et al., 2014; Kano and Fukudo, 2013; Kano et al., 2007, 2003; Moriguchi et al., 2007b; Paradiso et al., 2008; Reker et al., 2010; Silani et al., 2008; Zhang et al., 2011). Evidence for the role of the AI in particular for the ontogenesis of alexithymia was provided by a large-scale (N = 129) study of patients with traumatic brain injury (Hogeveen et al., 2016).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text