Elsevier

Neuropsychologia

Volume 53, January 2014, Pages 137-145
Neuropsychologia

Longitudinal processing speed impairments in males with autism and the effects of white matter microstructure

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.11.008Get rights and content

Highlights

  • A 13-year longitudinal study of processing speed in autism (ages 6–39 years).

  • Slower processing speed standard scores in autism compared to typical development.

  • Processing speed group differences in adult but not children's subtest raw scores.

  • Processing speed modestly predicted by whole-brain white matter in combined sample.

Abstract

The present study used an accelerated longitudinal design to examine group differences and age-related changes in processing speed in 81 individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) compared to 56 age-matched individuals with typical development (ages 6–39 years). Processing speed was assessed using the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-3rd edition (WISC-III) and the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-3rd edition (WAIS-III). Follow-up analyses examined processing speed subtest performance and relations between processing speed and white matter microstructure (as measured with diffusion tensor imaging [DTI] in a subset of these participants). After controlling for full scale IQ, the present results show that processing speed index standard scores were on average 12 points lower in the group with ASD compared to the group with typical development. There were, however, no significant group differences in standard score age-related changes within this age range. For subtest raw scores, the group with ASD demonstrated robustly slower processing speeds in the adult versions of the IQ test (i.e., WAIS-III) but not in the child versions (WISC-III), even though age-related changes were similar in both the ASD and typically developing groups. This pattern of results may reflect difficulties that become increasingly evident in ASD on more complex measures of processing speed. Finally, DTI measures of whole-brain white matter microstructure suggested that fractional anisotropy (but not mean diffusivity, radial diffusivity, or axial diffusivity) made significant but small-sized contributions to processing speed standard scores across our entire sample. Taken together, the present findings suggest that robust decreases in processing speed may be present in ASD, more pronounced in adulthood, and partially attributable to white matter microstructural integrity.

Introduction

The ability to process implicit social cues and filter essential signals from a complex array of stimuli requires fast-paced integration of a large number of sensory inputs. Therefore, processing speed, or the speed at which we are able to perceive and react to stimuli in the environment, is a fundamental cognitive ability. Clinically, individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) often demonstrate the need for increased time to process information and perform tasks, which appears to contribute to functioning in daily life. Research studies also have found evidence of slower processing speeds across the lifespan in individuals with ASD. On standardized Wechsler tests of intelligence, lower processing speed index scores have been reported in children, (Mayes and Calhoun, 2003, Mayes and Calhoun, 2008, Oliveras-Rentas et al., 2012, Wechsler, 2003) adolescents, and adults with ASD (Spek, Schatorjé, Scholte, & van Berckelaer-Onnes, 2009). These slower processing speeds were associated with more severe ASD communication symptoms in high-functioning children with ASD (Oliveras-Rentas et al., 2012) and were found to be predictive of educational achievements in math, reading, and writing (Assouline, Foley Nicpon, & Dockery, 2011). These observations and results suggest that slower processing speed may be a common difficulty experienced by individuals with ASD and may relate to core ASD features, as well as educational and occupational prospects.

Although processing speed difficulties have been consistently reported in persons with ASD, little is known regarding how processing speed develops and changes with age in this population. In individuals with typical development, cross-sectional and longitudinal studies suggest rapid increases in processing speed during the first year of life (Rose, Feldman, & Jankowski, 2002), followed by continued increases through childhood and adolescence (Kail, 2007, Kail and Ferrer, 2007). Although very little is known about typical processing speed changes in middle age, processing speed appears to decline and may be related to other age-related declines in cognitive functions in older-adult populations (Salthouse, 1996, Salthouse and Ferrer-Caja, 2003, Sliwinski and Buschke, 1999, Sternäng et al., 2008, Zimprich and Kurtz, 2013). In both typically developing and patient populations, age-appropriate development of processing speed may be a critical predictor of a number of important cognitive and outcome measures. For example, processing speed may be a key predictor of general intelligence in typically developing adolescents (Coyle, Pillow, Snyder, & Kochunov, 2011), and in individuals with schizophrenia, processing speed may predict vocational outcome and social function (Sánchez et al., 2009). Therefore, understanding the longitudinal development of processing speed in persons with ASD may be a key element to better understanding and accommodating the needs of persons with ASD throughout the lifespan.

A better understanding of processing speed development in ASD may also elucidate underlying neural substrates contributing to the etiology of ASD. DTI studies in individuals with typical development suggest that processing speed may be associated with the white matter microstructure of a number of white matter tracts across the brain, including the bilateral anterior corona radiata, bilateral superior fronto-occipital fasciculus, bilateral superior longitudinal fasciculus, bilateral internal capsule, bilateral posterior cingulum, right inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus, and bilateral posterior corona radiata (Bendlin et al., 2010). In individuals with traumatic brain injury, processing speed was related to white matter microstructure of the superior longitudinal fasciculus (Turken et al., 2008), ventral striatum (Shah et al., 2012), as well as the corpus callosum and centrum semiovale (Kourtidou et al., 2012). Further, decreases in white matter integrity of the corpus callosum were related to decreases in processing speed in children with multiple sclerosis (Bethune et al., 2011) and in children and adults with ASD (Alexander et al., 2007). These results provide converging evidence across distinct samples and methodologies that the white matter microstructure of a number of tracts is likely involved in processing speed.

Because of the number of tracts likely involved, it is possible that a metric of whole-brain white matter microstructure may predict processing speed more accurately than the white matter microstructure of any individual tract. Indeed, in older individuals with typical development, a measure of higher radial diffusivity (RD) across all lobes of the brain was found to be associated with executive function and processing speed measures (Jacobs et al., 2013). Furthermore, two studies have found that a composite white matter integrity factor (i.e., a combination of FA, RD, MD, and axial diffusivity (AD) across eight major tracts, or an aggregation of the white matter integrity of healthy looking white matter tracts) was more predictive of processing speed than when the individual tracts were treated as single predictors (Penke et al., 2010, Venkatraman et al., 2011). Given that white matter microstructure atypicalities have been commonly reported in ASD (see Travers et al., 2012 for a review), investigating white matter microstructure as a predictor of processing speed is essential to better understand the links between the behavioral and the neural manifestations of ASD.

To examine the intersection among possible processing speed differences in ASD, age-related development of processing speed, and white matter contributions to processing speed, the present study longitudinally examined age-related processing speed changes in 81 individuals with ASD and 56 age-matched individuals with typical development from childhood to mid-adulthood (6–39 years old). Additional analyses examined whether whole-brain white matter FA, MD, RD, and AD predicted processing speed in a subset of participants who underwent multiple DTI scans.

Section snippets

Design

As part of a 13-year longitudinal study, the current study employed an accelerated longitudinal design (i.e., cohort-sequential design) (Harezlak et al., 2005, Nesselroade and Baltes, 1979), which simultaneously measured individual longitudinal changes in processing speed across multiple age cohorts. Data were collected at four separate time points: Pre-Time 1 (henceforth called Time 0), Time 1, Time 2, and Time 3. Processing speed data were collected at Time 0, Time 1, and Time 3. DTI scans

Longitudinal group differences in processing speed index (PSI)

We tested for group differences in PSI scores and their longitudinal trajectories. PSI scores for each participant are presented in Fig. 2. A linear mixed-effects model analysis controlling for full scale IQ suggested slower processing speed in individuals with ASD, such that the group with ASD averaged 12 points lower on PSI scores than typically developing peers (see Table 2). These significant group differences were not accompanied by significant age-related changes in PSI from six to 39

Discussion

Using an accelerated longitudinal design, the present study examined group differences and age-related changes in processing speed in individuals with ASD compared to individuals with typical development from childhood into adulthood. The results yielded three important findings: (1) A significant processing speed standard score deficit in ASD that tends to be sustained from childhood to adulthood, (2) a significant and robust processing speed raw score deficit in the ASD group in the adult but

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health [RO1 MH080826 to JEL, ALA, NL, EDB; and RO1 MH084795 to JEL, NL], the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development [T32 HD07489 to BGT, and P30 HD003352 to the Waisman Center], the Hartwell Foundation [to BGT], and the Poelman Foundation [to EDB]. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institute of Mental

References (53)

  • B.B. Bendlin et al.

    White matter in aging and cognition: A cross-sectional study of microstructure in adults aged eighteen to eighty-three

    Developmental Neuropsychology

    (2010)
  • L.C. Chang et al.

    RESTORE: Robust estimation of tensors by outlier rejection

    Magnetic Resonance in Medicine 

    (2005)
  • Cook, P.A., Bai, Y., Nedjati-Gilanio, S., Seunarine, K.K., Hall, M.G., Parker, G.J., et al. (2006). Camino: Open-Source...
  • T.R. Coyle et al.

    Processing speed mediates the development of general intelligence (g) in adolescence

    Psychological Science

    (2011)
  • M. Dennis et al.

    Why IQ is not a covariate in cognitive studies of neurodevelopmental disorders

    Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society: JINS

    (2009)
  • K.A. Fournier et al.

    Motor coordination in autism spectrum disorders: A synthesis and meta-analysis

    Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

    (2010)
  • D. García-Villamisar et al.

    Supported employment improves cognitive performance in adults with autism

    Journal of Intellectual Disability Research

    (2007)
  • F. Happé et al.

    The weak coherence account: Detail-focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders

    Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

    (2006)
  • J. Harezlak et al.

    Individual and population penalized regression splines for accelerated longitudinal designs

    Biometrics

    (2005)
  • H.I.L. Jacobs et al.

    Association between white matter microstructure, executive functions, and processing speed in older adults: The impact of vascular health

    Human Brain Mapping

    (2013)
  • R.V. Kail

    Longitudinal evidence that increases in processing speed and working memory enhance children's reasoning

    Psychological Science

    (2007)
  • Robert V. Kail et al.

    Processing speed in childhood and adolescence: Longitudinal models for examining developmental change

    Child Development

    (2007)
  • P. Kourtidou et al.

    Centrum semiovale and corpus callosum integrity in relation to information processing speed in patients with severe traumatic brain injury

    The Journal of Head Trauma Rehabilitation

    (2012)
  • A. Leemans et al.

    The B-matrix must be rotated when correcting for subject motion in DTI data

    Magnetic Resonance in Medicine : (Official Journal of the Society of Magnetic Resonance in Medicine/Society of Magnetic Resonance in Medicine

    (2009)
  • C. Lord et al.

    The autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism

    Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

    (2000)
  • C. Lord et al.

    Autism diagnostic interview-revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders

    Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

    (1994)
  • Cited by (37)

    • Altered topological properties of white matter structural network in adults with autism spectrum disorder

      2022, Asian Journal of Psychiatry
      Citation Excerpt :

      White matter is the brain region that lies beneath the gray matter cortex and is composed of highly myelinated neuronal fibers that allow information to be communicated quickly across large brain areas (Zhao et al., 2022). Disrupted integrity of white matter tracts may influence information transfer between the distance brain areas (Howard et al., 2017; Travers et al., 2014), leading to brain dysfunction (Wang and Olson, 2018) and psychiatric diseases (Bu et al., 2020). Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) is a promising method that can identify the integrity of white matter tracts by the measurement of water diffusion movement in neural tissue.

    • Reduced white-matter integrity and lower speed of information processing in adolescents with mild and moderate neonatal hypoxic-ischaemic encephalopathy

      2020, European Journal of Paediatric Neurology
      Citation Excerpt :

      Among the interesting questions that remain to be answered is whether speed of information processing and/or white matter integrity can be used as trans-diagnostic markers of increased risk of developing psychopathology, known to be present among children and adolescents with HIE [7]. If this were the case, this would be consistent with previous research showing the involvement of reduced speed of information processing in various neurodevelopmental disorders such as attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder, dyslexia and autism [64–66], as well as studies linking speed of information processing and white matter integrity with a general factor of psychopathology [67–70]. Another key questions posed by our results is whether a reduction in speed of information processing can be explained by reduced functional connectivity, resulting from reduced integrity of long-range white-matter fibres or potentially reduced integrity of more local white-matter connections.

    • Relationship between brain stem volume and aggression in children diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder

      2017, Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders
      Citation Excerpt :

      According to this cutoff, 14 ASD participants were in the “high aggression” group, 31 ASD participants were in the “low aggression” group, and all 18 TD participants were in the “low aggression” group. TD participants were evaluated using ADOS, IQ, language tests, and parent interview to confirm that all had age appropriate verbal skills and no history of learning, developmental, cognitive, neurological, or neuropsychiatric problems (Alexander et al., 2007; Allen-Brady et al., 2010; Jantz et al., 2015; Prigge et al., 2013; Travers et al., 2014). See Table 1 for more detailed descriptions of participant recruitment and characteristics.

    • Multivariate characterization of white matter heterogeneity in autism spectrum disorder

      2017, NeuroImage: Clinical
      Citation Excerpt :

      The institutional review boards of the University of Utah and University of Wisconsin-Madison approved the study protocol and procedures. Participants consisted of 92 individuals with ASD and 57 typically developing controls (TDC), selected from a broader longitudinal study of brain development in autism and typical development (Lange et al., 2015; Travers et al., 2015b; 2015a; 2014; Zielinski et al., 2014). Consent was obtained from all adult participants, and both parental consent and participant assent were obtained for participants under the age of 18 years.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text