Elsevier

Neuropsychologia

Volume 51, Issue 10, August 2013, Pages 1925-1932
Neuropsychologia

Spatial bias in symbolic and non-symbolic numerical comparison in neglect

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.06.004Get rights and content

Highlights

  • The spatial nature of non-symbolic numerosity representation was tested.

  • Numerical comparison abilities were tested using symbolic and non-symbolic inputs.

  • Patients with or without left neglect were compared to healthy controls.

  • Only neglect patients showed a spatial-numerical bias in both tasks.

  • A common representational system for symbolic and non-symbolic inputs is suggested.

Abstract

When asked to bisect mentally numerical intervals, neglect patients show a displacement of the numerical midpoint similar to the one observed in physical line bisection. This spatial-numerical bias has been taken as evidence of the spatial nature of numerical magnitude representations. However, to date, neuropsychological studies in neglect patients have only used symbolic numerical material. Here, we compare the results of patients with right-hemisphere damage with and without unilateral left neglect and age-matched healthy control participants in two numerical comparison tasks using symbolic and non-symbolic materials, in order to determine whether the representation of non-symbolic numerosities was altered or not by the presence of neglect. When asked to judge if an Arabic digit or a sequence of flashed dots was smaller or larger than a reference value (i.e., 5), the responses of neglect patients to smaller magnitudes (i.e., 4) were impaired. Moreover, only neglect patients presented an asymmetrical distance effect (i.e., an enhanced effect only for stimuli of smaller numerical magnitude than the reference). These results provide the first direct evidence of a spatial bias in non-symbolic numerosity in neglect patients, and support the existence of common processing mechanisms and/or a representational system for symbolic and non-symbolic inputs.

Introduction

Interactions between number and space processing have been reported in many studies (for a review, see Hubbard, Piazza, Pinel, & Dehaene, 2005), which has led to the suggestion that numerical magnitudes may be represented spatially with small numbers on the left and large numbers on the right side of a mental number line (Dehaene, 1992). To test the spatial nature of this numerical representation, recent neuropsychological research has investigated numerical abilities in neglect patients. Unilateral spatial neglect is defined as a failure to report, respond to or orient towards stimuli in contra-lesional space (Heilman, 1979) that often occurs after cerebral lesions involving the posterior-inferior parietal and the premotor cortices (Vallar, 1998), and that could possibly arise from a disruption of fronto-parietal white matter pathways (Doricchi and Tomaiuolo, 2003, Doricchi et al., 2008). When asked to indicate the midpoint of a physical line, neglect patients show a significant rightward deviation (e.g., Binder et al., 1992, Marshall and Halligan, 1989, Pizzamiglio et al., 2000). Neglect is not restricted to the perception of physical space and may extend to representational space. Indeed, there is evidence that neglect patients fail to recall the left side of well-known places, depending on their imagined viewpoint (e.g., Bisiach & Luzzatti, 1978). Nevertheless, a double dissociation between perceptual and representational neglect has been observed, suggesting the existence of at least partly independent attentional mechanisms operating in the perceptual and imaginal space (Anderson, 1993, Guariglia et al., 1993).

In numerical interval bisection tasks (i.e., determining the midpoint of a numerical interval), left neglect patients showed a displacement of the midpoint towards large numbers, suggesting a deviation to the right part of the mental number line (Cappelletti et al., 2007, Hoeckner et al., 2008, Zorzi et al., 2002, Zorzi et al., 2006). A case study of a right unilateral neglect patient showing the opposite bias (i.e., underestimation rather than overestimation of the midpoint; Pia, Corazzini, Folegatti, Gindri, & Cauda, 2009) supports the idea of a distortion of numerical representation caused by a deviation of visuospatial attention (however, see below van Dijck, Gevers, Lafosse, Doricchi, & Fias, 2011 for a rightward bias in a right neglect patient). Moreover, it has recently been demonstrated that neglect patients who present prismatic adaptation as a consequence of having worn shifting prisms (Rossetti et al., 2004) or who have received leftward optokinetic stimulation1 (Priftis, Pitteri, Meneghello, Umiltà, & Zorzi, 2012), both techniques inducing visuo-spatial shifts of attention to the left, showed a reduction in the overestimation bias. When neglect patients had to judge a given number as smaller or larger than a fixed reference number, they processed numbers just smaller than the reference more slowly than those just larger (Vuilleumier, Ortigue, & Brugger, 2004): when asked to compare numbers to a standard reference of 5, the patients were slower to respond to 4 than to 6, while they were slower to respond to 6 than to 8 when the reference was 7. This asymmetrical distance effect implies that neglect does not alter the representation of numbers per se, but induces a failure to access the left side of the mental number line relative to the standard. Random dot kinetograms (RDK, a technique that induces a perception of movement without eye movements) to the left also reduced this spatial bias in symbolic comparison tasks (Salillas, Granà, Juncadella, Rico, & Semenza, 2009). Taken together, these findings suggest that numerical judgements are biased by impaired spatial processing, which provides strong evidence of a numerical representation with spatial properties.

However, the double dissociation between physical line and numerical interval bisection impairments found in some neglect and non-neglect patients (Aiello et al., 2013, Aiello et al., 2012, Doricchi et al., 2005; for a review, see Rossetti et al., 2011) questions the idea of a simple functional equivalence between the mental number line and the representation of physical space. Indeed, not all neglect patients show a numerical impairment and, conversely, some right brain-lesioned non-neglect patients show a spatial-numerical bias during mental bisection of number intervals. In the sample tested by Doricchi et al. (2005), only patients with lesions in the prefrontal cortex, a region known to support some working memory processes, showed a rightward numerical bias, suggesting that this region contributes to activating a numerical continuum in visuo-spatial working memory. An additional argument in favour of a working memory account comes from a case study of a patient with an extended left hemispheric lesion who presented spatial neglect for the right visual hemispace but left neglect for numbers (i.e., overestimation of the midpoint of number interval bisection; van Dijck et al., 2011). This patient had difficulty retrieving the first items of verbal sequences, suggesting that the overestimation in number interval bisection observed in neglect patients could originate in verbal working memory impairment for the initial items. Moreover, the role of working memory in spatial-numerical associations has been supported by several behavioural studies showing that the ordinal information stored in verbal working memory can overcome the spatial-numerical interaction based on magnitude (Fias et al., 2011, van Dijck and Fias, 2011).

The actual meaning of the spatial-numerical bias observed in neglect patients is thus unclear, as it could be due either to the spatial properties of a long-term representation or to order-related processes in short-term working memory. In this context, using non-symbolic stimuli may provide a significant way of disentangling these two possibilities. On the one hand, computational modelling suggests that symbolic and non-symbolic numerical inputs are coded by specific mechanisms, but are represented within a common numerical magnitude system (Verguts & Fias, 2004), possibly implemented in the left and right parietal cortices (Dormal et al., 2010, Holloway et al., 2010, Piazza et al., 2007, Santens et al., 2010, Venkatraman et al., 2005). Furthermore, various magnitudes (e.g., numerosity, length, duration, etc.) may be represented, irrespective of their mode of presentation, within a common magnitude processing system, possibly located in the parietal cortex (Bonato et al., 2012, Walsh, 2003). Accordingly, the spatial-numerical bias should occur when processing symbolic but also non-symbolic stimuli. Yet, despite a growing interest in the neuropsychological investigation of unilateral neglect in the numerical domain, the studies so far have only tested spatial-numerical biases with symbolic notations. To the best of our knowledge, only one study to date has attempted to demonstrate the existence of a spatial bias in the perception of non-symbolic numerosities in healthy participants (de Hevia & Spelke, 2009). In a line bisection task flanked with a 2-dot and a 9-dot array, a shift of the subjective midpoint of the line in the direction of the numerically larger set of dots was observed. However, this experiment has been criticised because some non-numerical parameters were not controlled (e.g., area; Gebuis & Gevers, 2011). The assumption that processing non-symbolic magnitudes involves spatial representations is thus still an open question. On the other hand, non-symbolic sequential comparison requires more working memory resources than symbolic comparison, as the traces of each single presented dot must be somehow kept active to compare the target sequence to the standard. Therefore, patients with lower verbal working memory capacities should perform globally worse in this demanding task than controls with preserved working memory abilities. Finally, according to a working memory account, a bias for processing numerical magnitudes smaller than a reference in neglect patients should occur concomitantly with verbal working memory impairment (Doricchi et al., 2005).

The present study thus aims to determine whether or not the spatial bias observed in neglect patients when comparing symbolic numbers extends to non-symbolic materials which would (i) support the idea of a common system for processing/representing symbolic and non-symbolic numerical magnitudes and its spatial nature, and (ii) assess the extent to which a working memory interpretation better accounts for the data. Patients with and without left neglect and healthy controls were tested in two numerical comparison tasks, using Arabic digits or sequences of flashed dots. Given prior studies using numerical comparison, a distance effect (i.e., an increase in the response latencies and/or the error rates as the numerical distance between the numbers being compared decreases; Moyer & Landauer, 1967) is expected for both symbolic and non-symbolic inputs in control groups. Neglect patients are expected to be slower in symbolic number comparison task when responding to digits that are just smaller than the standard (i.e., in the number range from 1 to 9 and for a given reference of 5, digit 4 should be more difficult to process for the neglect group). Thus, particular attention will be devoted to performance on numerical magnitudes close to the standard on both sides of the standard. If this difficulty is attributed to an impaired access to the numerical representation on the relative left of the standard value on an input-independent spatial magnitude representation system, a similar pattern should arise both in symbolic and non-symbolic numerical comparison tasks. An absence of spatial-numerical bias in the non-symbolic task in neglect patients would challenge the hypothesis of a common spatial representation of symbolic and non-symbolic numerical magnitude. Working memory capacities of each group will be compared to evaluate if verbal working memory deficit accounts for the spatial-numerical bias in the neglect group.

Section snippets

Participants

Fourteen patients with left-unilateral spatial neglect following right posterior hemispheric damage (hereafter N+; mean age: 58±11 years, 4 females, 13 right-handed) and eleven patients showing no sign of neglect (hereafter N−; mean age: 60±11 years, 3 females, all right-handed)2

Results

Trials for which the participants could give no response (50/3744 data points) were excluded from further analyses. The median response latencies (RLs) of correct answers and the mean error rate (ER) for each digit or dot sequence were computed for all the participants. Hereafter, the variable Response will be used to categorise the stimuli as being smaller or larger than the fixed standard. The variable Distance (from 1 to 4) corresponds to the numerical distance between the stimuli and the

Discussion

The performance of neglect patients in numerical tasks using symbolic stimuli had previously been interpreted as supporting the idea of a spatial representation of numbers. However, this bias was observed with symbolic inputs only and could be accounted for by serial order-related processes in verbal working memory. In this study, patients with right-hemisphere damage with or without left neglect and healthy control participants were tested in comparison tasks with symbolic and non-symbolic

Conclusion

The observed impairment in processing non-symbolic numerosities smaller than a standard extends previous observations on symbolic material. Our findings indicate an impairment of both symbolic and non-symbolic numerical processes caused by spatial neglect, suggesting that neglect affects a process common to both types of input. Hence, our results support recent models that propose a common numerical magnitude system for symbolic numbers and non-symbolic numerosities and, moreover, the

Acknowledgements

This study was supported by grant FSR09-CWS/09.285 from the Fonds Spéciaux de Recherche of the Université catholique de Louvain (Belgium). NM is a research fellow, MP is a research associate, and VD is a postdoctoral researcher at the National Fund for Scientific Research (Belgium). We thank Annick Beerten, Caroline Cools, Nathalie Depoorter, Aurélie Dewulf, Hoëlle Genot, Rufus Hipaux, Gwenaelle Mary, Julie Nootens, Laurence Robaye, Cécile Prairial, Caroline Swalens and Sophie Verplancke for

References (65)

  • V Dormal et al.

    Numerosity-duration interference: a Stroop-experiment

    Acta Psychologica

    (2006)
  • W Fias et al.

    How number is associated with space? The role of working memory

  • T Gebuis et al.

    Numerosities and space; indeed a cognitive illusion! A reply to de Hevia and Spelke (2009)

    Cognition

    (2011)
  • S H Hoeckner et al.

    Impairments of the mental line for two-digits numbers in neglect

    Cortex

    (2008)
  • I D Holloway et al.

    Common and segrated neural pathways for the processing of symbolic and nonsymbolic numerical magnitude: an fMRI study

    NeuroImage

    (2010)
  • G Kerkhoff

    Modulation and rehabilitation of spatial neglect by sensory stimulation

  • J C Marshall et al.

    When right goes left: an investigation of line bisection in a case of visual neglect

    Cortex

    (1989)
  • T G Philippi et al.

    Multisensory temporal numerosity judgment

    Brain Research

    (2008)
  • L Pia et al.

    Mental number line disruption in a right-neglect patient after a left-hemisphere stroke

    Brain and Cognition

    (2009)
  • M Piazza et al.

    A magnitude code common to numerosities and number symbols in human intraparietal cortex

    Neuron

    (2007)
  • L Pizzamiglio et al.

    Psychophysical properties of line bisection and body midline perception in unilateral neglect

    Cortex

    (2000)
  • Y Rossetti et al.

    Neglect around the clock: dissociating number and spatial neglect in right brain damage

  • E Salillas et al.

    Leftward motion restores number space in neglect

    Cortex

    (2009)
  • G Vallar

    Spatial hemineglect in humans

    Trends in Cognitive Science

    (1998)
  • J-P van Dijck et al.

    A working memory account for spatial-numerical associations

    Cognition

    (2011)
  • J-P van Dijck et al.

    Non-spatial neglect for the mental number line

    Neuropsychologia

    (2011)
  • V Venkatraman et al.

    Neural correlates of symbolic and non-symbolic arithmetic

    Neuropsychologia

    (2005)
  • P Vuilleumier et al.

    The number space and neglect

    Cortex

    (2004)
  • V Walsh

    A theory of magnitude: common cortical metrics of time, space, and quantity

    Trends in Cognitive Science

    (2003)
  • M Zorzi et al.

    The spatial representation of numerical and non-numerical sequences: evidence from neglect

    Neuropsychologia

    (2006)
  • M Aiello et al.

    No inherent left and right side in human ‘mental number line’: evidence from right brain damage

    Brain

    (2012)
  • B Anderson

    Spared awareness for the left side of the internal visual images in patients with left-sided extrapersonal neglect

    Neurology

    (1993)
  • Cited by (17)

    • Effects of attentional shifts along the vertical axis on number processing: An eye-tracking study with optokinetic stimulation

      2022, Cognition
      Citation Excerpt :

      Considering that eye movements and attentional orienting are deeply related (premotor theory of attention: Casarotti, Lisi, Umiltà, & Zorzi, 2012; Rizzolatti, Riggio, Dascola, & Umiltà, 1987), these findings further support the idea that the mental representation of numbers recruits visuospatial attention. Neuropsychological studies have found impaired number-space associations in patients with unilateral spatial neglect, a syndrome characterized by attentional deficits in the contralesional side of space following brain-damage (e.g., Aiello et al., 2012; Masson, Pesenti, & Dormal, 2013; Van Dijck, Gevers, Lafosse, & Fias, 2012; Zorzi, Priftis, & Umiltà, 2002; Zorzi et al., 2012; see Umiltà, Priftis, & Zorzi, 2009, for a review of earlier studies). Impairment in accessing the spatial representation of numbers in neglect patients indicates that number and space are causally linked by visuospatial attention, and it suggests that cognitive and neural mechanisms might be shared between the two domains.

    • Number space is made by response space: Evidence from left spatial neglect

      2021, Neuropsychologia
      Citation Excerpt :

      To gain an insight into the functional origin of the SNARC, several authors have assessed this effect in right brain damaged (RBD) patients affected by attentional neglect for the left side of space (N+; Barbaranelli, 2007; Lecce et al., 2015; Silvetti et al., 2016). Following the original work by Vuilleumier et al. (2004), these studies have homogenously demonstrated that in Magnitude SNARC tasks N+ show a typical asymmetry in Reaction Times (RTs), so that RTs to numbers that are immediately smaller than a magnitude reference are slowed down compared to numbers that are immediately larger than the same reference (Salillas et al., 2009; van Dijck et al., 2012; Zorzi et al., 2012; Masson et al., 2013; van Dijck and Doricchi 2019). As an example, when the reference is “5” N+ patients are significantly slower at deciding that “4” is smaller than “5” than at deciding that “6” is larger than “5”.

    • Numerical cognition: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging, transcranial magnetic stimulation and brain-damaged patients studies

      2019, NeuroImage: Clinical
      Citation Excerpt :

      Several group studies in right brain-damaged (RBD) patients have also indicated greater impairment in numerical magnitude processing than calculation (Dellatolas et al., 2001; Rosselli and Ardilla, 1989; Warrington and James, 1967). For instance, Masson et al. (2013) showed that RBD patients with neglect were impaired in symbolic and non-symbolic numerical order judgment tasks with small quantities, suggesting that neglect prevents from orienting attention to the left of the mental number line (see also Masson et al., 2015). The goal of this article was to test the main predictions derived from the TCM (for a summary, see Table 1).

    • Dissociation between line bisection and mental-number-line bisection in healthy adults

      2015, Neuropsychologia
      Citation Excerpt :

      This suggests that the mental bisection of number intervals is poorly and not systematically influenced by pathological biases of spatial attention. Nonetheless, and in contrast to this, studies that have investigated the performance of RBD patients with left spatial neglect in magnitude comparison-SNARC tasks requiring the association of left/right spatial response codes with number magnitudes have provided more homogenous results (Vuilleumier et al., 2004; van Dijck et al., 2011b; Zorzi et al., 2012; Masson et al., 2013). These show that patients with neglect have a clear RT asymmetry for numbers immediately adjacent to the numerical reference: RTs are faster for numbers higher than the reference (e.g. 6 when the reference is 5) and slower for numbers lower than the reference (e.g. 4 when the reference is 5, 6 when the reference is 7).

    • Causal role of spatial attention in arithmetic problem solving: Evidence from left unilateral neglect

      2014, Neuropsychologia
      Citation Excerpt :

      The absence of interaction with the magnitude of the answer indicates that the role of spatial attention in arithmetic operations is determined by the relative position of the answers with respect to the first operand (i.e., always smaller for subtraction and always larger for addition) rather than by their absolute position in a putative mental number line. The view that attention shifts are defined by the relative rather than absolute position of the numbers to be processed is further corroborated by the performance of neglect patients in standard number comparison tasks where a deficit to mentally orient their attention towards the representation of numbers located on the left side of a given reference number was observed whatever the magnitude of the reference (Masson et al., 2013; Vuilleumier et al., 2004). Because left unilateral neglect did not interfere with the subtraction of small numbers (i.e., single-digit numbers), it is reasonable to assume that spatial attention is called into action mainly when larger numbers (i.e., two-digit numbers) are subtracted.

    • Neuropsychological methods: Patient studies

      2023, Experimental Methods in Embodied Cognition: How Cognitive Psychologists Approach Embodiment
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text