Elsevier

Neuropsychologia

Volume 50, Issue 3, February 2012, Pages 357-366
Neuropsychologia

White matter integrity and reaction time intraindividual variability in healthy aging and early-stage Alzheimer disease

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.11.024Get rights and content

Abstract

Aging and early-stage Alzheimer disease (AD) have been shown to be associated with increased RT intraindividual variability (IIV, as reflected by the coefficient of variation) and an exaggeration of the slow tail of the reaction time (RT) distribution in attentional control tasks, based on ex-Gaussian analyses. The current study examined associations between white matter volume, IIV, and ex-Gaussian RT distribution parameters in cognitively normal aging and early-stage AD. Three RT attention tasks (Stroop, Simon, and a consonant-vowel odd-even switching task) in conjunction with MRI-based measures of cerebral and regional white matter volume were obtained in 133 cognitively normal and 33 early-stage AD individuals. Larger volumes were associated with less IIV and less slowing in the tail of the RT distribution, and larger cerebral and inferior parietal white matter volumes were associated with faster modal reaction time. Collectively, these results support a role of white matter integrity in IIV and distributional skewing, and are consistent with the hypothesis that IIV and RT distributional skewing are sensitive to breakdowns in executive control processes in normal and pathological aging.

Highlights

► Aging/dementia tied to greater intraindividual variability/distributional skewing. ► We relate variability to total and regional white matter volumes. ► Specific associations observed in frontal and parietal regions. ► Larger volume associated with less variability/skewing in healthy/pathological aging.

Introduction

An important challenge in the cognitive neuroscience of aging involves understanding the associations between cognitive and structural changes in the aging brain (Raz, 2004, Raz and Rodrigue, 2006) and distinguishing these changes in the context of normal and pathological aging (Johnson, Storandt, Morris, & Galvin, 2009). Recent work suggests that examination of white matter integrity and potentially more sensitive metrics of cognitive performance beyond mean reaction time may facilitate understanding of brain-behavior associations in aging (Gunning-Dixon and Raz, 2000, Johnson et al., 2010, MacDonald et al., 2009, MacDonald et al., 2006). The present paper focuses on the association between regional white matter volume and both cognitive intraindividual variability (IIV) and reaction time distribution parameters in cognitively normal and pathological aging.

White matter integrity has been shown to be compromised in both healthy aging and early-stage Alzheimer disease (AD; Gunning-Dixon et al., 2009, Johnson et al., 2010). First, consider the evidence regarding healthy aging. This work has revealed more anterior declines in white matter in healthy aging that may accelerate with time (e.g., Bartzokis et al., 2003, Gunning-Dixon et al., 2009, Head et al., 2004, Salat et al., 2009). Furthermore, altered regulation of functional activity in frontal regions may be tied to white matter integrity in aging (see Grady, 2008, for a review). Additionally, the importance of white matter integrity is revealed in its association with reduced functional connectivity of brain regions in aging. For example, Andrews-Hanna et al. (2007) reported a positive association between reduced functional connectivity and reduced white matter integrity, even after controlling for the influence of age. Associations between white matter integrity and cognitive performance have also been reported in the aging literature (Gunning-Dixon et al., 2009, Raz et al., 2007, Raz et al., 2008).

Turning to pathological aging, there may be a differential vulnerability in more posterior regions in AD (Head et al., 2004, Kavcic et al., 2008, Salat et al., 2009). Recently, Bartzokis (2009) has suggested that AD may begin with compromised myelin. This is supported by evidence demonstrating strong links between hippocampal atrophy, an early region of decline in AD, and atrophy of critical white matter regions (Villain et al., 2010; but see Bai et al., 2009). Furthermore, frontal, temporal, corpus callosal, and parietal white matter integrity have been associated with memory and executive function in AD (Amar et al., 1996, Anstey et al., 2007, Huang and Auchus, 2007, Kavcic et al., 2008).

Although Rabbitt (1966) early on noted the importance of reaction time (RT) variability in the study of aging, there has been a burgeoning interest in this topic in the recent literature (see Hultsch, Strauss, Hunter, & MacDonald, 2008, for a review of variability in aging). Typically, examinations of cognitive functioning tend to rely on mean RT, a measure of central tendency that assumes stability within individuals or groups. Treating the mean as the dependent measure relegates RT variability to random noise, ignoring potentially systematic variations that may lead to a better understanding of cognitive and neurobiological processes. For purposes of the present paper, we focus on inconsistency, a type of IIV indicating fluctuations in RT task performance over very short intervals (i.e., variability on a trial-to-trial level). One needs to be concerned that the variability is above and beyond the increase in overall mean performance, since there is a strong relation between mean performance and SDs. Thus, researchers have turned to the coefficient of variation (CoV; SD/mean), which is emerging as a standard measure of inconsistency, due to its low bias from mean reaction time, relatively easy calculation, and high association with other measures of inconsistency (Hultsch, MacDonald, Hunter, Levy-Bencheton, & Strauss, 2000).

There is accumulating evidence indicating that aging is associated with increased IIV (Hultsch et al., 2002, Hultsch et al., 2008, Nesselroade and Salthouse, 2004), with increased variability linked to poorer cognitive performance (Hultsch & MacDonald, 2004). The increased variability appears to be particularly strong in more complex, attention-demanding tasks (Bielak et al., 2010, Lövdén et al., 2007, MacDonald et al., 2003). Rabbitt, Osman, and Moore (2001) have argued that higher IIV indicates less effective behavioral and attentional control that may occur via increasingly inefficient neural and network activation, and this perspective is supported by neurocomputational models (Li et al., 2006a, Li et al., 2006b). IIV has proven to be a useful marker of cognitive change, and can reliably distinguish between younger and older adults, even after correcting for age-related changes in central tendency (Bielak et al., 2010, Hultsch et al., 2008). Interestingly, Duchek et al. (2009) and Dixon et al. (2007) reported that the CoV from cognitive attention tasks can also discriminate between healthy and pathological aging, and can even identify cognitively normal individuals at risk for developing dementia (i.e., carriers of an Apolipoprotein ɛ4 allele). Indeed, Duchek et al. (2009) suggested that the increased IIV may reflect impaired attentional control and executive functioning.

There is currently limited research on the association between white matter integrity and RT inconsistency, although the studies that have been reported have indicated significant associations. Higher IIV, as measured by standard deviation, has been linked to smaller cerebral white matter (Walhovd & Fjell, 2007) as well as prefrontal, temporal and parietal white matter volumes (Ullén, Forsman, Blom, Karabanov, & Madison, 2008), and greater prefrontal white matter hyperintensities in healthy adults (Bunce et al., 2007). Associations have also been reported between volumetric estimates of the corpus callosum and IIV in individuals with mild cognitive disorders (Anstey et al., 2007). However, to our knowledge, this earlier research has not been extended to older adults. Only one past study (Walhovd & Fjell, 2007) has examined adults beyond 64 years of age and this study focused on total cerebral white matter volume, constraining generalizations about the relationships among aging, variability, and white matter integrity. Furthermore, none of the previous studies have examined this relationship in early-stage AD. Finally, previous studies have tended to use non-demanding cognitive tasks, such as visual oddball tasks (Walhovd & Fjell, 2007), simple RT tasks (Anstey et al., 2007), or isochronous tapping (Ullén et al., 2008). In the present study, the use of standard attentional control tasks as a basis for the calculation of RT IIV allowed us to draw more robust conclusions regarding the association between variability, control systems, and white matter integrity in older adults.

The importance of white matter integrity becomes increasingly important in light of recent work on brain networks. For example, connectivity in the default network, a group of frontal and parietal brain regions that become active and spontaneously correlate in the absence of an explicit task (Raichle et al., 2001), has been central in a number of recent studies of aging and early stage AD. Indeed, both aging and AD are associated with alterations of the default network (Andrews-Hanna et al., 2007, Bai et al., 2008, Damoiseaux et al., 2008, Grecius et al., 2004). Typically, the default network is anticorrelated with attentional networks, where one network is suppressed while the other is active. Kelly, Uddin, Biswal, Castellanos, and Milham (2008) reported that dysregulation of default and executive networks (i.e., lack of a strong anticorrelation) is associated with increased IIV. A breakdown in white matter integrity may disrupt both within and cross network correlations, thereby producing the observed relation reported in the Kelly et al. (2008) study.

There is a rich history showing that the characteristics of RT distributions are sensitive to various aspects of cognition (Luce, 1986, Ratcliff, 1978, Ratcliff, 1979), such as attentional control and/or drift rate in diffusion models (Heathcote et al., 1991, Ratcliff, 1978, Ratcliff, 1979, Spieler et al., 1996, Spieler et al., 2000, Tse et al., 2010). We have chosen to fit the Ex-Gaussian theoretical distribution to capture the characteristics of the RT distributions in the current data. The ex-Gaussian distribution reflects the convolution of a Gaussian and an exponential distribution. There are three parameters obtained from ex-Gaussian analyses: μ and σ, which capture the mean and standard deviation of the Gaussian portion, respectively; and τ, which reflects both the mean of the exponential portion of the distribution. Changes in μ reflect a shifting of the modal portion of the RT distribution, whereas changes in τ typically reflect an increase in the slow tail of the RT distribution. Critically, the algebraic sum of μ and τ equals the mean of the RT distribution, which enables one to make contact with the mean-dominated literature. The Ex-Gaussian distribution has been shown to fit RT data extremely well (Ratcliff, 1979), and has yielded interesting findings on the nature of human cognition (see Balota & Yap, 2011). Interestingly, it is possible to observe two RT distributions with equivalent means, but with opposing distributional parameters that cancel each other out, highlighting the importance of examining the properties of underlying reaction time distributions. For example, consider the congruency effect (the RT difference in naming the color of a congruent word, e.g., RED presented in red, and the color of a neutral word, e.g., DEEP presented in red) in the classic Stroop task. This effect is quite small and sometimes nonsignificant at the level of the mean. However, Heathcote et al. (1991) found that the relatively small congruency effect was due to the opposing influences of a shortening of the modal portion of the distribution (μ) in the congruent condition and a lengthening of the tail (τ) relative to the neutral condition (also see Spieler et al., 2000), both of which would be undetected at the level of the mean.

More recently, distributional analyses have helped to clarify differences between healthy and pathological aging. For example, Tse et al. (2010) recently found that healthy aging had clear effects on both μ and τ in a set of attentional control tasks, whereas early stage AD only had an additional effect on τ. Balota et al. (2010) have recently shown that the slow tail of the RT distribution in the Stroop task may also be useful in predicting convergence from a cognitive normal state to early stage AD across a 12 year longitudinal follow up. Furthermore, τ has been shown to be more strongly associated with working memory measures than μ or σ (Schmiedek et al., 2007, Tse et al., 2010), and thus it is possible that τ may be more related to the white matter integrity that underlies executive/attentional control abilities (see Balota et al., 2010, Breteler et al., 1994, Grieve et al., 2007, Gunning-Dixon et al., 2009, Raz et al., 2008). To our knowledge, no studies have examined the relationship between the parameters of RT distributional analysis and regional volume.

The present study examines (a) the relationship between RT IIV/distributional parameters and white matter volume in targeted brain regions, and (b) whether there are differential behavioral-brain associations between healthy aging and early-stage AD. Specifically, given the evidence of associations between IIV and white matter integrity observed in younger adult samples using less-engaging cognitive tasks (Anstey et al., 2007, Bunce et al., 2007, Ullén et al., 2008, Walhovd and Fjell, 2007), and the disruption of the default mode network in healthy aging and early stage AD (Andrews-Hanna et al., 2007, Damoiseaux et al., 2008), we examined the CoV and RT distributional parameters in attentional control tasks, and the relation between these estimates with cerebral white matter volume as well as targeted volumes in prefrontal and default-mode white matter regions. Moreover, because cognitive variability and distributional skewing may be particularly associated with frontal regions (see MacDonald et al., 2009, MacDonald et al., 2006), those frontal regions important in executive function such as ventral/dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, the superior frontal gyrus, and anterior cingulate may show increased sensitivity to fluctuations in CoV and the exponential estimate from ex-Gaussian analyses. In addition, targeted default-mode regions such as the precuneus, posterior cingulate, and the inferior parietal lobule (see Raichle et al., 2001) may show an association with variability and/or distributional parameters because the anticorrelatons between the default and executive networks has also demonstrated sensitivity to IIV in younger adults (Kelly et al., 2008). Finally, we expected stronger associations between regional white matter volume and cognitive performance in early-stage AD, as there are increasing breakdowns in white matter integrity and IIV, consistent with recent work by Anstey and colleagues (2007), in which they observed a stronger relationship between white matter volume and RT variability in MCI patients.

Section snippets

Participants

One hundred and thirty-three cognitively normal individuals (87 females), aged 46–96 (M = 68.0; SD = 9.6) and 33 individuals with early-stage AD (17 females), aged 61–88 (M = 76.6; SD = 6.1) were recruited from the Knight Alzheimer's Disease Research Center at Washington University. Participants were classified based on the interview-based Clinical Dementia Rating scale (CDR; Berg, 1988), a validated measure highly effective in detecting the earliest stages of dementia (Morris, 1993), as cognitively

CoV

Descriptive statistics for cognitive and brain structural measures are summarized in Table 2. Age accounted for a significant amount of variance in CoV (ΔR2 = .12, F(1, 158) = 23.04, p < .001), such that older individuals had larger CoV scores. CDR status was also a significant predictor of the CoV composite (ΔR2 = .19, F(1, 157) = 46.79, p < .001), such that early-stage AD individuals had larger CoV composites than cognitively normal individuals.

As shown in Fig. 1, after controlling for age and CDR

Discussion

Recent literature has established important links between cognitive performance and brain structure in healthy and pathological aging, including an emphasis on white matter integrity in executive and default network regions (e.g., Anstey et al., 2007, Bartzokis, 2004, Bartzokis et al., 2003, Bielak et al., 2010, Raz et al., 2008). The current study demonstrated a robust relationship between RT IIV and total cerebral and regional white matter volumes in both healthy older adults and in

Disclosure statement

The authors and their institution have no conflicts of interest related to this work. The sources of financial support were NIH grants P50 AG05861, P01 AG 03991, PO1 AGO26276 and National Institute of General Medical Sciences grant T32-GM81739-02. The data contained in this manuscript have not previously been published, nor has the manuscript been submitted elsewhere, nor will it be submitted elsewhere while under review. Appropriate ethical guidelines were followed with regard to the treatment

Acknowledgments

We thank the Clinical Core of the Knight Alzheimer's Disease Research Center at Washington University for the clinical assessments and the Imaging Core for the structural MRI data, and Martha Storandt for helpful comments. Supported by NIH Grants P50 AG05861, P01 AG 03991, and PO1 AGO26276. Jonathan Jackson was supported by National Institute of General Medical Sciences Grant T32-GM81739-02.

References (100)

  • X. Han et al.

    Reliability of MRI-derived measurements of human cerebral cortical thickness: The effects of field strength, scanner upgrade and manufacturer

    NeuroImage

    (2006)
  • J. Jovicich et al.

    MRI-derived measurements of human subcortical, ventricular and intracranial brain volumes: Reliability effects of scan sessions, acquisition sequences, data analyses, scanner upgrade, scanner vendors and field strengths

    NeuroImage

    (2009)
  • V. Kavcic et al.

    White matter integrity linked to functional impairments in aging and early Alzheimer's disease

    Alzheimer's and Dementia

    (2008)
  • A.M.C. Kelly et al.

    Competition between functional brain networks mediates behavioral variability

    NeuroImage

    (2008)
  • S.-C. Li et al.

    Neuromodulation of associative and organizational plasticity across the life span: Empirical evidence and neurocomputational modeling

    Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews

    (2006)
  • S.-C. Li et al.

    A neurocomputational model of stochastic resonance and aging

    Neurocomputing

    (2006)
  • M. Lövdén et al.

    Within-person trial-to-trial variability precedes and predicts cognitive decline in old and very old age: Longitudinal data from the Berlin Aging Study

    Neuropsychologia

    (2007)
  • S.W.S. MacDonald et al.

    Intra-individual variability in behavior: Links to brain structure, neurotransmission and neuronal activity

    Trends in Neurosciences

    (2006)
  • D.H. Mathalon et al.

    Correction for head size in brain-imaging measurements

    Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging

    (1993)
  • N. Raz et al.

    Differential aging of the brain: Patterns, cognitive correlates and modifiers

    Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews

    (2006)
  • D.H. Salat et al.

    Regional white matter volume differences in nondemented aging and Alzheimer's disease

    NeuroImage

    (2009)
  • K.B. Walhovd et al.

    White matter volume predicts reaction time instability

    Neuropsychologia

    (2007)
  • K. Amar et al.

    The effect of white matter low attenuation on cognitive performance in dementia of the Alzheimer type

    Age and Ageing

    (1996)
  • K.J. Anstey

    Sensorimotor and forced expiratory volume as correlates of speed, accuracy, and variability in reaction time performance in late adulthood

    Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition

    (1999)
  • D.A. Balota et al.

    Predicting conversion to dementia of the Alzheimer's type in a healthy control sample: The power of errors in Stroop color naming

    Psychology and Aging

    (2010)
  • D.A. Balota et al.

    Moving beyond the mean in studies of mental chronometry: The power of response time distributional analyses

    Current Directions in Psychological Science

    (2011)
  • G. Bartzokis

    Alzheimer's disease as homeostatic responses to age-related myelin breakdown

    Neurobiology of Aging

    (2009)
  • G. Bartzokis et al.

    White matter structural integrity in healthy aging adults and patients with Alzheimer disease: A magnetic resonance imaging study

    Archives of Neurology

    (2003)
  • L. Berg

    Clinical dementia rating (CDR)

    Psychopharmacol Bulletin

    (1988)
  • L. Berg et al.

    Clinicopathologic studies in cognitively healthy aging and Alzheimer's disease: Relation of histologic markers to dementia severity, age, sex, and apolipoprotein E genotype

    Archives of Neurology

    (1998)
  • A.A.M. Bielak et al.

    Intraindividual variability is related to cognitive change in older adults: Evidence for within-person coupling

    Psychology and Aging

    (2010)
  • M.M. Breteler et al.

    Cognitive correlates of ventricular enlargement and cerebral white matter lesions on magnetic resonance imaging. The Rotterdam Study

    Stroke

    (1994)
  • D. Bunce et al.

    Blocks in choice responding as a function of age and physical fitness

    Psychology and Aging

    (1993)
  • J.S.A. Carriere et al.

    Age trends for failures of sustained attention

    Psychology and Aging

    (2010)
  • A.E. Cavanna et al.

    The precuneus: A review of its functional anatomy and behavioural correlates

    Brain

    (2006)
  • J. Cheyne et al.

    Anatomy of an error: A bidirectional state model of task engagement/disengagement and attention-related errors

    Cognition

    (2009)
  • D. Cousineau et al.

    Fitting distributions using maximum likelihood: Methods and packages

    Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers

    (2004)
  • J.S. Damoiseaux et al.

    Reduced resting-state brain activity in the default network in normal aging

    Cerebral Cortex

    (2008)
  • R.S. Desikan et al.

    Automated MRI measures identify individuals with mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease

    Brain

    (2009)
  • R.A. Dixon et al.

    Neurocognitive markers of cognitive impairment: Exploring the roles of speed and inconsistency

    Neuropsychology

    (2007)
  • J.M. Duchek et al.

    The utility of intraindividual variability as an early marker for Alzheimer's disease

    Neuropsychology

    (2009)
  • C. Fennema-Notestine et al.

    Feasibility of multi-site clinical structural neuroimaging studies of aging using legacy data

    Neuroinformatics

    (2007)
  • B.A. Fischl et al.

    Automatically parcellating the human cerebral cortex

    Cerebral Cortex

    (2004)
  • C. Grady

    Cognitive neuroscience of aging

    Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences

    (2008)
  • M.D. Grecius et al.

    Default-mode network activity distinguishes Alzheimer's disease from healthy aging: Evidence from functional MRI

    Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences

    (2004)
  • S.M. Grieve et al.

    American Journal of Neuroradiology

    (2007)
  • F.M. Gunning-Dixon et al.

    Aging of cerebral white matter: A review of MRI findings

    International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry

    (2009)
  • F. Gunning-Dixon et al.

    The cognitive correlates of white matter abnormalities in normal aging, a quantitative review

    Neuropsychology

    (2000)
  • D. Head et al.

    Differential vulnerability of anterior white matter in nondemented aging with minimal acceleration in dementia of the Alzheimer type: Evidence from diffusion tensor imaging

    Cerebral Cortex

    (2004)
  • D. Head et al.

    Neuroanatomical and cognitive mediators of age-related differences in episodic memory

    Neuropsychology

    (2008)
  • Cited by (93)

    • Ex-Gaussian analysis of simple response time as a measure of information processing speed and the relationship with brain morphometry in multiple sclerosis

      2022, Multiple Sclerosis and Related Disorders
      Citation Excerpt :

      There is evidence that ex-Gaussian parameters differentially predict volumetric brain changes in some neurological disorders, suggesting clinical value associated with adopting this mathematical modeling approach. For example, smaller brain volumes correlate with larger estimates of tau and sigma in aging and Alzheimer's disease (Jackson et al., 2012; Schumacher et al., 2019). Other exploratory analyses have identified relationships across all ex-Gaussian estimates and brain volume in Lewy Body Dementia (Schumacher et al., 2019).

    • Beta-amyloid moderates the relationship between cortical thickness and attentional control in middle- and older-aged adults

      2022, Neurobiology of Aging
      Citation Excerpt :

      These differences in activation are often tied to task characteristics such as difficulty (Kennedy et al., 2015; Schneider-Garces et al., 2010), and it is unclear whether this activity represents dedifferentiation, compensation, or both (Park and Reuter-Lorenz, 2009). The associations between cognition and brain structure are more straight-forward, with prior work in aging cohorts demonstrating that worse cognitive performance is related to greater atrophy measured with structural MRI (Buckner, 2004; Cardenas et al., 2011; Kramer et al., 2007; Persson et al., 2005; Rusinek et al., 2003; Vyhnalek et al., 2014) and lower average values of white matter integrity assessed using DTI (Charlton et al., 2006; Jackson et al., 2012; Madden et al., 2009; Voineskos et al., 2012; Walhovd and Fjell, 2007). The links between behavior and the brain are often interpreted as representing typical, or healthy aging, as participants are cognitively normal.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text