Elsevier

NeuroImage

Volume 50, Issue 2, 1 April 2010, Pages 753-763
NeuroImage

fMRI-adaptation evidence of overlapping neural representations for objects related in function or manipulation

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.036Get rights and content

Abstract

Sensorimotor-based theories of semantic memory contend that semantic information about an object is represented in the neural substrate invoked when we perceive or interact with it. We used fMRI adaptation to test this prediction, measuring brain activation as participants read pairs of words. Pairs shared function (flashlight–lantern), shape (marble–grape), both (pencil–pen), were unrelated (saucer–needle), or were identical (drill–drill). We observed adaptation for pairs with both function and shape similarity in left premotor cortex. Further, degree of function similarity was correlated with adaptation in three regions: two in the left temporal lobe (left medial temporal lobe, left middle temporal gyrus), which has been hypothesized to play a role in mutimodal integration, and one in left superior frontal gyrus. We also found that degree of manipulation (i.e., action) and function similarity were both correlated with adaptation in two regions: left premotor cortex and left intraparietal sulcus (involved in guiding actions). Additional considerations suggest that the adaptation in these two regions was driven by manipulation similarity alone; thus, these results imply that manipulation information about objects is encoded in brain regions involved in performing or guiding actions. Unexpectedly, these same two regions showed increased activation (rather than adaptation) for objects similar in shape. Overall, we found evidence (in the form of adaptation) that objects that share semantic features have overlapping representations. Further, the particular regions of overlap provide support for the existence of both sensorimotor and amodal/multimodal representations.

Introduction

Many theories of semantic memory posit that the meanings of concepts can be described as patterns of activation that are distributed over semantic features (e.g., Allport, 1985, Barsalou, 1999, Tyler et al., 2000). A benefit of this kind of architecture is that relationships between concepts can be captured via overlapping representations (McRae et al., 1997, Masson, 1995). Some distributed theories of semantic memory further suggest that semantic information about an object is represented in the neural substrate that is invoked when we perceive and/or interact with that object. Specifically, these sensorimotor-based theories suggest that the meaning of a (concrete) concept is not distributed in an amodal, unitary semantic system, but instead different aspects of meaning are stored in physically distal networks, according to the modality in which the information was acquired (e.g., Warrington and McCarthy, 1987, Glenberg, 1997, Barsalou, 1999). Amodal theories (e.g., Caramazza and Shelton, 1998), on the other hand, do not posit that representations are sensorimotor-based and so need not predict that concepts are situated in modality specific cortices.

Regardless of whether it is posited that meanings are distributed over features in an amodal system or across multiple, modality specific systems, all distributed models of semantic memory assume that the representations of concepts that share features overlap. This means that activating a particular concept should also partially activate other concepts that share its features. The semantic priming effect, wherein identifying a target word is facilitated when it is preceded by a (conceptually) related prime word (e.g., Meyer and Schvaneveldt, 1971) can therefore be interpreted as support for distributed models. However, semantic priming studies typically use primes and targets from the same semantic category, and as many have pointed out (e.g., Kellenbach et al., 2000), category co-exemplars are usually related in multiple ways (e.g., crayon and pencil are both thin, oblong, used for marking paper, and grasped with the thumb and the second and third fingers). Therefore, although the results of these studies show that semantically related words partially activate each other, they cannot identify which of these features are responsible for the facilitation effect. Identifying the responsible features would help distinguish between sensorimotor and amodal distributed models of semantic memory; if concepts that are related via sensorimotor features partially activate each other, this would suggest that their meanings are represented (at least in part) as patterns of activation over sensorimotor-based attributes.

A handful of studies have explicitly manipulated the semantic relationship between primes and targets (e.g., Schreuder et al., 1984). Most of these studies explored whether semantic priming would be obtained when primes and targets have the same shape or function (function is defined here as the purpose for which an object is used, e.g., flashlight and lantern have the same function), and both shape and function priming have been observed1 (Schreuder et al., 1984, Flores d'Arcais et al., 1985, Taylor and Heindel, 2001). Semantic priming has also been observed for objects that are manipulated (i.e., interacted with) similarly (e.g., piano and typewriter, which are both tapped with the fingers [Myung et al., 2006]). Results from visual world eyetracking studies, in which preferential fixations were observed on objects related (in function, shape, or manipulation) to a named object, are also consistent with the hypothesis that related objects have overlapping representations (Yee, et al., under review; Myung et al., 2006). Behavioral studies therefore suggest that objects with similar functions, manipulations or shapes do in fact have partially overlapping representations. If this is true, then this overlap should be instantiated at the neural level. The experiment reported here examines whether feature overlap is instantiated at the neural level by using an fMRI adaptation paradigm that permits identification of the particular brain regions in which the overlap is located.

The assumption underlying the fMRI adaptation paradigm is that repeated presentation of the same visual or verbal stimulus results in reduced fMRI signal levels in brain regions that process that stimulus, either because of neuronal “fatigue” (e.g., firing-rate adaptation) or because the initial activation of a stimulus' representation is less neurally efficient than subsequent activation (see Grill-Spector et al., 2006 for a review). In a typical fMRI adaptation experiment, stimuli are presented which are either identical (which produces an adaptation/reduced hemodynamic response) or completely different (producing a recovery response). However, it is possible to use stimuli pairs that are related, rather than identical, and several recent fMRI studies of semantically related word pairs have found less activation for related than unrelated word pairs, predominantly in temporal and/or inferior frontal cortices (Kotz et al., 2002, Rossell et al., 2003, Rissman et al., 2003, Copland et al., 2003, Giesbrecht et al., 2004, Matsumoto et al., 2005, Gold et al., 2005, Tivarus et al., 2006, Kuperberg et al., 2007, Bedny et al., 2008).

The adaptation paradigm can also be employed while varying the level of stimulus similarity (e.g., Kourtzi and Kanwisher, 2001, Epstein et al., 2003, Fang et al., 2005) in order to obtain a measure of neurally perceived difference: the greater the similarity, the greater the expected adaptation. Because of this sensitivity to similarity, the adaptation paradigm is a natural fit for examining whether, as predicted by distributed models, semantically related objects have overlapping representations; for if they do, when one object's representation is activated, semantically related objects should also be partially activated. Thus the presentation of semantically related objects should produce adaptation, with the greater the relatedness, the greater the adaptation. Two recent fMRI studies found just such an ordered effect of relatedness (but c.f. Raposo et al., 2006). In Wheatley et al. (2005) subjects silently read pairs of words which were either unrelated in meaning, semantically related, or identical. In left ventral temporal cortex and in anterior left inferior frontal gyrus, the greatest activity was found for unrelated word pairs, less for semantically related pairs, and least of all for identical pairs. Similarly, in Wible et al. (2006), subjects heard pairs of words that were either strongly connected (in that they shared many associates), weakly connected (sharing fewer associates), or unrelated. In bilateral posterior superior and middle temporal cortex, activation was greatest for unrelated pairs, less for weakly connected, and the least of all for strongly connected pairs.

The fact that many of these semantic adaptation studies found adaptation in the inferior frontal and middle temporal gyri in particular could be interpreted as support for amodal representations, as it has been suggested that these areas underlie amodal semantic processing (e.g., Postler et al., 2003). Crucially, however, none of these prior adaptation studies attempted to test specific sensorimotor features. Therefore, they do not address whether concepts' representations may also be comprised of sensorimotor features that are represented in sensorimotor areas.

Brain imaging studies that have addressed particular semantic features have demonstrated that when retrieving information about a specific attribute of an object (e.g., its color or shape), brain areas in, or just anterior to, those implicated in perceiving that attribute become active (at least for color, shape, and manipulation; for a review, see Thompson-Schill et al., 2006). While it is possible that these sensorimotor regions were activated as a consequence of tasks that directed attention to their corresponding features, there is some evidence to the contrary: even under conditions that do not require attending to manipulation information (i.e., under passive viewing) pictures of tools produce greater activation in left ventral premotor and left posterior parietal cortex than non-manipulable objects (Chao and Martin, 2000). The fact that motor regions were automatically activated when tools were viewed implies that these motor regions are part of tools' conceptual representations. This is consistent with the notion that concepts are represented in or near perceptual cortices. However, these studies do not address whether objects that share semantic features have overlapping (rather than nearby) representations.

If as sensorimotor-based theories suggest, the meaning of a (concrete) concept is distributed over the multiple brain regions involved in perceiving and/or interacting with the object, then conceiving of an object should automatically activate these sensorimotor regions. An object's shape and the way it is manipulated can both be directly linked to sensorimotor information. On the other hand, function, in the way we define it (the purpose for which an object is used, e.g., flashlight and lantern share the same function) does not by itself have a direct sensorimotor correlate. It is therefore possible that shape and manipulation similarity will be reflected in adaptation in regions devoted to processing object form and to motor programming respectively, but that function similarity will be instantiated in regions that have been hypothesized to represent more abstract, higher order relationships.

We use an adaptation paradigm to focus on two features in particular: function and shape, asking specifically whether pairs of objects with similar functions and/or shapes have overlapping neural representations (as indicated by eliciting a smaller neural response than unrelated pairs), and also whether the extent of any such overlap can be predicted by the degree of similarity. Because there is considerable variability across such pairs in manipulation similarity (e.g., things that are similar in both function and shape tend to be manipulated more similarly, while things similar in shape but not function tend not to be), we also examine whether objects that are manipulated similarly have overlapping representations.

Section snippets

Subjects

Subjects were 30, right handed, monolingual, native speakers of American English, aged 20-33, from the University of Pennsylvania and Drexel University communities. They were paid $10 for participating.

Stimuli

We selected an initial set of 195 word pairs. Seventy-two were from Thompson-Schill and Gabrieli (1999), and an additional one hundred twenty-three were developed using the MRC Psycholinguistic Database (Coltheart, 1981) and the University of South Florida Free Association Norms (Nelson et al.,

Behavioral results

Property verification response times were analyzed. Accuracy information was not calculated because norms for what constituted a correct response for each probe-item pair combination were not available. Because the task was to decide whether the probe word applied to either word in the pair, for “yes” responses, it is conceivable that when responding to the probe participants only considered one of the items. We therefore included in the analysis only trials in which subjects answered “no” (76%

Discussion

In the current study, we used an fMRI adaptation paradigm to explore the nature of semantic representations. In particular, we asked whether objects with similar functions and/or shapes or manipulations have overlapping neural representations. Another goal was to investigate the extent to which the representations of objects with similar sensorimotor features (shapes or manipulations) overlap in sensorimotor cortices, and the extent to which the representations of objects with similar abstract

Conclusions

We found adaptation for words similar in function and shape, and also that degree of adaptation was predicted by degree of manipulation similarity in two regions, and by degree of function similarity in three additional regions. Specifically, adaptation correlated with manipulation in dorsal stream regions that have been implicated in motor imagery and motor behavior, suggesting that manipulation information is encoded in sensorimotor areas. Similarity in function, on the other hand (a feature

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by NIH Grant (R01MH70850) awarded to Sharon Thompson-Schill and by a Ruth L. Kirschstein NRSA Postdoctoral Fellowship awarded to Eiling Yee. We are grateful to Geoff Aguirre for assistance with data analysis, and to Katherine White, Sarah Drucker and members of the Thompson-Schill lab for helpful comments.

References (67)

  • GoodaleM.A. et al.

    Separate visual pathways for perception and action

    Trends Neurosci.

    (1992)
  • GraftonS.T. et al.

    Premotor cortex activation during observation and naming of familiar tools

    NeuroImage

    (1997)
  • Grill-SpectorK. et al.

    Repetition and the brain: neural models of stimulus-specific effects

    Trends Cogn. Sci.

    (2006)
  • KellenbachM.L. et al.

    Visual semantic features are activated during the processing of concrete words: event-related potential evidence for perceptual semantic priming

    Cogn. Brain Res.

    (2000)
  • KotzS.A. et al.

    Modulation of the lexical-semantic network by auditory semantic priming: an event-related functional MRI study

    NeuroImage

    (2002)
  • MatsumotoA. et al.

    Linking semantic priming effect in functional MRI and event-related potentials

    NeuroImage

    (2005)
  • MyungJ. et al.

    Playing on the typewriter and typing on the piano: manipulation features of man-made objects

    Cognition

    (2006)
  • OliverR.T. et al.

    Remembrance of things touched: how sensorimotor experience affects the neural instantiation of object form

    Neuropsychologia

    (2009)
  • RaposoA. et al.

    Repetition suppression and semantic enhancement: an investigation of the neural correlates of priming

    Neuropsychologia

    (2006)
  • RossellS.L. et al.

    Brain activation during automatic and controlled processing of semantic relations: a priming experiment using lexical-decision

    Neuropsychologia

    (2001)
  • RossellS.L. et al.

    The anatomy and time course of semantic priming investigated by fMRI and ERPs

    Neuropsychologia

    (2003)
  • Thompson-SchillS.L. et al.

    A neural basis for category and modality specificity of semantic knowledge

    Neuropsychologia

    (1999)
  • TylerL.K. et al.

    Conceptual structure and the structure of concepts: a distributed account of category specific deficits

    Brain Lang.

    (2000)
  • WibleC.G. et al.

    Connectivity among semantic associates: an fMRI study of semantic priming

    Brain Lang.

    (2006)
  • AllportD.A.

    Distributed memory, modular subsystems and dysphasia

  • BarsalouL.W.

    Perceptual symbol systems

    Behav. Brain Sci.

    (1999)
  • BednyM. et al.

    Semantic adaptation and competition during word comprehension

    Cereb. Cortex

    (2008)
  • BinkofskiF. et al.

    A parieto-premotor network for object manipulation: evidence from neuroimaging

    Exp. Brain Res.

    (1999)
  • BurockM.A. et al.

    Randomized event-related experimental designs allow for extremely rapid presentation rates using functional MRI

    NeuroReport

    (1998)
  • CanessaN. et al.

    The different neural correlates of action and functional knowledge in semantic memory: an FMRI study

    Cereb. Cortex

    (2008)
  • CaramazzaA. et al.

    Domain-specific knowledge systems in the brain the animate–inanimate distinction

    J. Cogn. Neurosci.

    (1998)
  • ColtheartM.

    The MRC Psycholinguistics database

    Q. J. Exp. Psychol.

    (1981)
  • DaleA.M.

    Optimal experimental design for event-related fMRI

    Hum. Brain Mapp.

    (1999)
  • Cited by (38)

    • Bilateral Intraparietal Activation for Number Tasks in Studies Using Adaptation Paradigm: A Meta-analysis

      2022, Neuroscience
      Citation Excerpt :

      Nevertheless, this paradigm’s advantages overweigh this limitation. Consequently, the adaptation paradigm has been widely used in neuroscientific research to assess the neuronal responses to a specific stimulus, especially in visual perception and object recognition studies (Winston et al., 2004; Fang et al., 2005; Sawamura et al., 2006; Cavina-Pratesi et al., 2010; Cohen Kadosh et al., 2010; Weiner et al., 2010; Yee et al., 2010). It has also been used in space processing, motion detection (Ashida et al., 2007; Chong et al., 2008; Brozzoli et al., 2011) and language research (Gold et al., 2005; Dufor and Rapp, 2013; Yu et al., 2015).

    • Role of features and categories in the organization of object knowledge: Evidence from adaptation fMRI

      2016, Cortex
      Citation Excerpt :

      Specifically, based on a definition of function feature as the purpose of use (e.g., Canessa et al., 2008; Moss et al., 1995; Yee et al., 2011), the function feature overlap for taxonomically related word pairs (e.g., saw – axe) referred to whether the two objects share the same purpose of use (e.g., cutting) and for thematically related word pairs (e.g., saw – wood) whether the purpose of use for one of the two objects is to perform an action on the other object (e.g., a saw is to cut wood). Following the instructions used in Yee et al. (2010, 2011) and Moss et al. (1995), we collected ratings from 20 subjects for all related and unrelated word pairs on a 1–7 scale for function similarity: “rate the following pairs of objects according to how similar their functions are (i.e., the purpose of use)”, and collected ratings from another 20 subjects to assess function relatedness: “rate the following pairs of objects according to how likely the purpose of use for one of the two objects is to perform action on the other one”. As expected, the taxonomically related word pairs had higher function similarity ratings compared to the thematically related pairs [t1 (19) = 6.84, p < .001; t2 (74) = 16.79, p < .001], and the thematically related word pairs had higher function relatedness ratings compared to the taxonomically related word pairs [t1 (19) = 13.53, p < .001; t2 (74) = 14.58, p < .001] (see Table 1).

    • Compositionality and the angular gyrus: A multi-voxel similarity analysis of the semantic composition of nouns and verbs

      2015, Neuropsychologia
      Citation Excerpt :

      After all, the legacy of model-theoretic semantics has concerned itself primarily with formalizing “the metaphysics of truth in natural language” rather than the various constraints on language processing or the representations of concepts (Seuren, 2009). Cognitive neuroscientists are often more explicitly interested in semantics as it deals with binding sensorimotor features of object-concepts, or how different categories of objects are represented with regard to action-oriented events, e.g., function vs. manipulability (Yee et al., 2010), etc. For instance, early attempts to define “category-specific” regions of cortex using lesion studies provided evidence that damage to ATL was associated with deficits specific to the knowledge of living things (Gainotti, 1996), while damage to left temporo-parietal junction affected knowledge of man-made artifacts (e.g. wrench, hammer, etc. which, interestingly, are often also verbs) (Tranel et al., 1997).

    • Deeper insights into semantic relations: An fMRI study of part-whole and functional associations

      2014, Brain and Language
      Citation Excerpt :

      This difference might induce greater uncontrolled variability in the semantic or visual or structural features across pictures. However, as shown in the current and also earlier studies, when conditions are carefully matched on linguistic parameters, the use of different stimulus sets between conditions should not have a considerable effect while excluding the possibility of repetition or habituation effects (e.g. Abel et al., 2009; Alario et al., 2000; Kircher et al., 2009; Sass et al., 2009b; Wible et al., 2006; Yee, Drucker, & Thompson-Schill, 2010). An important requirement is a careful matching procedure as conducted in the current study.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text