Elsevier

Neuroscience Letters

Volume 543, 24 May 2013, Pages 42-46
Neuroscience Letters

Severity of generalized social anxiety disorder correlates with low executive functioning

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2013.02.059Get rights and content

Highlights

  • We evaluate neurocognitive functions of patients with social anxiety disorder.

  • The WCST performance was lower than that of healthy controls.

  • Performance of the WCST correlates with the LSAS score.

  • Social anxiety disorder has low executive function correlates with the severity.

Abstract

To evaluate neurocognitive functions of patients with social anxiety disorder (SAD) without comorbidity using neuropsychological assessments and to investigate the relation between neurocognitive functions and clinical severity of SAD, this study assessed 30 SAD patients (10 female, 20 male) without comorbidity and 30 healthy subjects matched on gender, education level, and age. The neuropsychological assessment consisted of the Wisconsin card sorting test (WCST), the continuous performance test, the trail-making test, the word fluency test, and the auditory verbal learning test. On the WCST, patients showed lower performance than healthy controls did. The Liebowitz Social Anxiety Scale score correlated significantly with the numbers of perseverative errors of the WCST, although the State anxiety score of State-Trait Anxiety Inventory and the Beck Depression Inventory – Second Edition score showed no correlation with neuropsychological test scores. Results show that the executive functioning of patients with SAD was low and that the low functioning correlates with the SAD symptom severity.

Introduction

Social anxiety disorder (SAD) is characterized by a persistent fear of one or more social or performance situations. The prevalence of this disorder is high: approximately 1.4–12.1% of the population meets DSM-IV TR criteria [4] for SAD over their lifetime [1], [19], [20], [27], [30]. A person with SAD, although recognizing that the fear is unreasonable or excessive, usually cannot resolve it and deflect attention away from the fear.

According to the cognitive model of SAD, self-focused attention is an important factor in maintaining the illness. A person with SAD tends to increase access to self-referent negative thoughts and feelings that interfere with performance, thereby preventing the individual from observing external information that might disprove the thoughts and feelings of patients with SAD [11]. Presumably, this inflexibility of informational processes is based on neurocognitive dysfunction. The dysfunction of executive functioning, attention and memory is frequently reported in relation to anxiety disorders [10]. Executive functioning is defined as a set of general-purpose control mechanisms, often linked to the pre-frontal cortex of the brain, that regulate the dynamics of human cognition and action [28]. Unlike psychotic or mood disorders, little is known about the neurocognitive impairment of anxiety disorders except for obsessive–compulsive disorder and posttraumatic stress disorder. The impairment of executive functioning, visual memory, attention and processing speed has been reported as associated with obsessive–compulsive disorder. The dysfunction of executive functioning, attention, verbal and visual memory has been reported as associated with posttraumatic stress disorder. However, few reports describe studies of SAD patients’ neurocognitive functions [10], [13].

Several studies have examined threat biases by which individuals with SAD devote selective attention to socially threatening situations [26], [32], [41], [42]. Results do not necessarily support threat biases. Nevertheless, few studies have targeted cognitive functions independently of social context. Among such studies that have assessed potential cognitive impairment in SAD, one found that patients with SAD were more impaired than healthy controls in terms of verbal memory [3]. Another study revealed that patients with SAD had lower executive functioning and visual memory scores than healthy controls had [12]. Based on these studies, patients with SAD might show low performance in tasks of verbal memory, visual memory, and executive functioning. However, results of these studies were not congruent, perhaps because of comorbidities, especially major depressive disorder.

Generally speaking, SAD is frequently comorbid with major depressive disorder, with estimated frequency of 44–70% [9], [23], [33]. In connection with major depressive disorder, many reports have described that memory, learning, attention, motor function, and problem-solving might be affected [5], [40]. Therefore, it is possible that depressive symptoms affect the neurocognitive functions of patients with SAD. Actually, one study demonstrated that verbal memory impairment is correlated with the Beck Depression Inventory score [7]. The impairment of verbal memory might be influenced by comorbid depression. Moreover, another study set their exclusion criterion as 16 points (and above) on the Hamilton rating scale for depression (HAM-D) [15], presented the possibility that some of their subjects had mild depression.

However, one report has described that comorbid depressed versus non-depressed SAD patients respond uniquely to stress in terms of their neuropsychological function [14]. We were unable to find a report in the relevant literature describing the evaluation of a relation between clinical severity and neurocognitive functions in patients with SAD who had few depressive symptoms, but who nevertheless had severe social anxiety symptoms for which they had sought clinical treatment.

For this study, we recruited outpatients without comorbidity to exclude effects of depressive symptoms. This study was conducted to evaluate neurocognitive functions in SAD patients without comorbidity, and to investigate the relation between clinical severity and neurocognitive functions.

Section snippets

Participants

From outpatients at the Department of Psychiatry, Hokkaido University Hospital, 30 patients (10 female, 20 male; mean age (S.D.), 23.9 (6.7) years) were recruited. Using the Mini-international neuropsychiatric interview [31], [34], psychiatrists who had at least 10 years of clinical experience and who were blind to this study diagnosed all patients as having generalized SAD in DSM-IV TR [4]. At recruitment, the HAM-D and the global assessment of functioning (GAF) were checked. Patients were

Results

Backgrounds of healthy controls and patients with SAD are presented in Table 1. No significant difference in sex, age (F1, 58 = 1.26, P = 0.266), JART full scale IQ (F1, 58 = 0.00, P = 0.957) and years of education (F1, 58 = 0.26, P = 0.614) was found. The LSAS Total score (F1, 58 = 33.53, P < 0.001) was significantly higher for the patients with SAD than for the healthy controls. The STAI state anxiety score (F1, 58 = 30.24, P < 0.001) and BDI-II score (F1, 58 = 37.68, P < 0.001) were also higher than those of

Discussion

The results of this study indicate that patients with SAD have low performance relative to healthy controls in terms of the category achievement and the perseverative error on WCST. Furthermore, the SAD severity correlates with the perseverative errors on WCST, but tends to correlate negatively with the category achievement on WCST, although other clinical conditions show no correlation with the scores. Results suggest that the low WCST performance correlates with the severity of SAD, even if

Conclusions

Results presented in this report indicate that patients with SAD have low executive functioning. Furthermore, this low functioning is correlated with SAD severity, even if other clinical conditions, including depressive symptoms, are considered. Although the correlation supports a parallel relation between SAD severity and low executive function, longitudinal studies that intervene in executive function or SAD symptoms must be conducted to clarify the relation of the cognitive impairment and

Acknowledgments

No conflict of interest exists in connection with this paper for any author. We are grateful to all participants and testers, and especially to Yui Takahashi and Chikako Katayama.

References (43)

  • G.J. Asmundson et al.

    Neurocognitive function in panic disorder and social phobia patients

    Anxiety

    (1994)
  • American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic statistical manual of mental disorders fourth edition Text Revision....
  • M.P. Austin et al.

    Cognitive deficits in depression: possible implications for functional neuropathology

    Br. J. Psychiatry

    (2001)
  • A.T. Beck et al.

    Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory-Second Edition

    (1996)
  • A.T. Beck et al.

    An inventory for measuring depression

    Arch. Gen. Psychiatry

    (1961)
  • P. Bollini et al.

    Effectiveness of antidepressants. Meta-analysis of dose-effect relationships in randomised clinical trials

    Br. J. Psychiatry

    (1999)
  • T.A. Brown et al.

    Reliability of DSM-IV anxiety and mood disorders: implications for the classification of emotional disorders

    J. Abnorm. Psychol.

    (2001)
  • D.M. Clark et al.

    Cognitive model of social phobia

  • L.J. Cohen et al.

    Specificity of neuropsychological impairment in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a comparison with social phobic and normal control subjects

    J. Neuropsychiatry Clin. Neurosci.

    (1996)
  • F. Ferreri et al.

    Current research on cognitive aspects of anxiety disorders

    Curr. Opin. Psychiatry

    (2011)
  • M. Hamilton

    A rating scale for depression

    J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry

    (1960)
  • Cited by (42)

    • Effects of cranial electrotherapy stimulation with novel in-ear electrodes on anxiety and resting-state brain activity: A randomized double-blind placebo-controlled trial

      2021, Journal of Affective Disorders
      Citation Excerpt :

      In people with anxiety, impairment of executive function is most evident, and specifically cognitive inflexibility is concurrently and predictively associated with anxiety symptoms (Derakshan and Eysenck, 2009; Eysenck et al., 2007). This is demonstrated by less category achievement or more perseverative errors on the WCST in patients with anxiety disorders (Caselli et al., 2004; Fujii et al., 2013; Goodwin and Sher, 1992; Tempesta et al., 2013), which thus sensitively reflects the anxiety-related cognitive deficit. The WCST yields several scores for assessment, such as total correct (TC), perseverative error (PT), and categories completed (CC).

    • A transdiagnostic examination of anxiety and stress on executive function outcomes in disorders with social impairment

      2021, Journal of Affective Disorders
      Citation Excerpt :

      Empirical studies in clinical cohorts examining the relationship between anxiety, stress and EF have also shown mixed results (Fujii et al., 2013; Heeren, Maurage, & Philippot, 2015). In a cohort diagnosed with SAD, a negative relationship was reported between symptom severity of social anxiety and performance on a set-shifting task (Fujii et al., 2013). Levels of social anxiety did not influence other EF (fluency, set-switching) and non-EF measures (STM).

    • The role of social anxiety in autism and the broader autism phenotype: Evidence from childhood through adulthood

      2021, Factors Affecting Neurodevelopment: Genetics, Neurology, Behavior, and Diet
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text