Review
The auditory dorsal pathway: Orienting vision

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.04.005Get rights and content

Abstract

A particularly prominent model of auditory cortical function proposes that a dorsal brain pathway, emanating from the posterior auditory cortex, is primarily concerned with processing the spatial features of sounds. In the present paper, we outline some difficulties with a strict functional interpretation of this pathway, and highlight the recent trend to understand this pathway in terms of one that uses acoustic information to guide motor output towards objects of interest. In this spirit, we consider the possibility that some of the auditory spatial processing activity that has been observed in the dorsal pathway may actually be understood as a form of action processing in which the visual system may be guided to a particular location of interest. In this regard, attentional orientation may be considered a low-level form of action planning. Incorporating an auditory-guided motor aspect to the dorsal pathway not only offers a more holistic account of auditory processing, but also provides a more ecologically valid perspective on auditory processing in dorsal brain regions.

Introduction

The ‘decade of the brain’ brought with it many advances in our understanding of the auditory cortical system. By the early 1990s it had become evident that cells in the auditory cortex were not functionally homogeneous. Single-cell research in non-human primates established that the primary auditory cortex (i.e., AI) was surrounded by a ‘belt’ area, which in turn was bordered laterally by a ‘parabelt’ region (Kosaki et al., 1997, Morel et al., 1993, Rauschecker, 1997). Whereas neurons in the core (i.e., AI) responded best to simple auditory stimuli such as pure tones, those in the belt and parabelt responded best to more complex sounds (e.g., vocalizations and bands of noise, Rauschecker et al., 1995). Most remarkably, caudal but not rostral neurons within these areas, were shown to be particularly sensitive to the location of sounds (Benson et al., 1981, Kaas and Hackett, 2000, Leinonen et al., 1980, Morel et al., 1993, Rauschecker, 1998, Recanzone et al., 2000, Tian et al., 2001, Vaadia et al., 1986), whereas more rostrally positioned neurons in the lateral belt and beyond were more sensitive to non-spatial acoustic qualities such as conspecific vocalizations (Rauschecker and Tian, 2000, Tian et al., 2001). Beyond auditory cortex, connections from the caudal parabelt regions were found to extend to the ventral inferior parietal (VIP) cortex of the monkey (Lewis and Van Essen, 2000) as well as reciprocally into the caudal principal sulcus (area 46) and frontal eye fields (area 8a, Hackett et al., 1999, Romanski et al., 1999a, Romanski et al., 1999b). By comparison, the rostral belt region was found to have connections with the frontal pole (area 10), rostral principal sulcus (area 46), and ventral prefrontal areas (areas 12 and 45, Romanski and Goldman-Rakic, 2002). Taken as a whole, these results were interpreted as forming the basis of a domain-specific model of auditory processing whereby auditory non-spatial (i.e., “what”) and spatial (i.e., “where”) information were processed by ventral and dorsal brain pathways, respectively (i.e., the “what–where” model or WW model, Kaas and Hackett, 1999, Rauschecker and Tian, 2000, Romanski et al., 1999b).

This auditory model bore many similarities to the “what–where” model of the visual cortical system developed over a decade earlier in which two visual pathways were defined as emanating from the striate cortex: a ventral stream crucial for visual identification of objects, and an occipitotemporal–parietal pathway crucial for appreciating the spatial relationships among objects as well as for the visual guidance of movements towards objects in space (Ungerleider and Haxby, 1994, Ungerleider and Mishkin, 1982). Support for that model was derived from research demonstrating that lesions to monkey inferior temporal cortex resulted in deficits on pattern, object, or colour discrimination tasks, but not on visuospatial tasks such as visually guided reaching or relative distance judgments (see Ungerleider and Mishkin, 1982). Lesions to the posterior parietal cortex, on the other hand, did not seem to affect visual discrimination performance but did affect visuospatial performance. Physiological data also supported the distinction (Desimone and Ungerleider, 1989, Maunsell and Newsome, 1987), as did data from humans (Haxby et al., 1991, von Cramon and Kerkhoff, 1993).

Not long after the auditory findings in animals were reported, a similar pattern of auditory processing was revealed in humans. In one of the first experiments to offer double dissociative evidence in neurologically intact humans, we presented functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) participants with noiseburst sounds that varied in pitch and perceived location (Alain et al., 2001). Relative to when listeners were asked to attend to the location features of the sounds, attention to the sounds’ pitch elicited greater blood-oxygen-level dependent (BOLD) hemodynamic activity in the auditory cortex and inferior frontal gyrus. In contrast, attending to the location properties elicited relatively greater activity in the participants’ posterior temporal lobe, superior parietal lobe and superior frontal sulcus. Event-related potentials recorded while the listeners carried out these tasks also revealed differential task effects over the anterior and posterior temporal regions. These, along with many other neuroimaging and patient observations, strongly argued in favour of a dual pathway model of human auditory cortical processing, often termed the auditory ‘what–where’ model (see Fig. 1, Ahveninen et al., 2006, Alain et al., 2008, Altmann et al., 2007, Anourova et al., 2001, Arnott et al., 2004, Arnott et al., 2005, Barrett and Hall, 2006, Belin and Zatorre, 2000, Bushara et al., 1999, De Santis et al., 2007, Degerman et al., 2006, Deouell et al., 2007, Maeder et al., 2001, Schröger and Wolff, 1997, Tardif et al., 2008, Tata and Ward, 2005, Thiran and Clarke, 2003, Weeks et al., 1999, Zatorre et al., 2002).

Despite its popularity, the auditory WW model has not been without criticism (Belin and Zatorre, 2000, Hall, 2003, Recanzone and Cohen, 2010). One concern has been that a distinction based on ‘what’ and ‘where’ features of a sound tends to be an oversimplification of the data and does not provide an adequate functional account of how the auditory system and the brain, in general, operates. Moreover, it has become apparent that non-spatial auditory processing can also elicit activation in the dorsal pathway, albeit often to a lesser extent than does spatial processing (cf. Arnott et al., 2004, Gifford and Cohen, 2005, Husain and Nachev, 2007, Renier et al., 2009), and that, in some cases, the parietal neurons that are activated by auditory spatial tasks can be the same as those activated by non-spatial tasks (Gifford and Cohen, 2005). For example, our meta-analysis of auditory neuroimaging studies demonstrated that unlike regions of the superior frontal sulcus (SFS) that were driven exclusively by auditory spatial processing, over 40% of auditory studies considered to be ‘non-spatial’ in nature (i.e., the auditory stimuli were only ever delivered from one location and the listener's task did not involve any kind of localization or spatial judgment), reported significant functional activity in the inferior parietal lobe (IPL) region in addition to the virtually 100% of auditory ‘spatial’ processing studies that were examined (Arnott et al., 2004). Accordingly, while it is evident that the auditory dorsal pathway, and the IPL in particular, is activated any time a relative auditory spatial judgment has to be made, it is also apparent that the IPL is involved in more than just creating auditory spatial maps.

Section snippets

Re-examining the dorsal pathway

In recent years, a subtle but important variation on the role of the auditory dorsal pathway has become evident across disparate domains of auditory research. In particular, there has been an increasing emphasis on the role that the dorsal pathway plays in instructing action, or guiding motor output based on auditory information. Before reviewing these data, it is important to acknowledge the visuo-motor theory from which some of this work draws its inspiration.

Popularized in the 1990s by

Sound localization: calls to action?

Look with thine ears” – Act 4. Scene VI, King Lear, William Shakespeare

Given the wide range of evidence outlined above implicating dorsal brain regions as being involved in auditory-motor integration, it may be useful to consider spatial localization and the ‘where’ pathway from an auditory-motor perspective. In their paper, Kubovy and Van Valkenburg (2001) argued that auditory spatial localization system could be thought of as one that was “in the service” of visual orientation and that the

Dorsal pathway and attentional orienting

In addition to its role in eye movement programming, it is clear from what we have reviewed that a large proportion of auditory activity in the dorsal stream is associated with attentional processing. The inclusion of an attentional component does not invalidate the idea that auditory localization serves to inform/orient the visual system. In reality, spatial attention and eye movement programming can be thought of as part and parcel of the same orienting mechanism. For instance, while strong

Concluding remarks

Viewing the auditory dorsal pathway as one that guides or instructs motor output based on acoustic information offers certain advantages. As has been shown, it rather nicely encapsulates models of speech processing (Hickok and Poeppel, 2007, Rauschecker and Scott, 2009), and helps explain why non-spatial sound processing can stimulate activation in dorsal areas. In keeping with the notion of goal-directed actions, our literature review has shown how auditory spatial processing within the dorsal

Acknowledgements

The authors wish to thank two anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments on an earlier version of this manuscript.

References (209)

  • S. Carlile et al.

    The localisation of spectrally restricted sounds by human listeners

    Hear. Res.

    (1999)
  • M.E. Cohen et al.

    Saccade latency in children and adults: effects of warning interval and target eccentricity

    J. Exp. Child Psychol.

    (1977)
  • Y.E. Cohen

    Multimodal activity in the parietal cortex

    Hear. Res.

    (2009)
  • Q.N. Cui et al.

    Eye position and cross-sensory learning both contribute to prism adaptation of auditory space

    Prog. Brain Res.

    (2008)
  • J.C. Culham et al.

    The role of parietal cortex in visuomotor control: what have we learned from neuroimaging?

    Neuropsychologia

    (2006)
  • M. D’Esposito et al.

    The role of prefrontal cortex in sensory memory and motor preparation: an event-related fMRI study

    Neuroimage

    (2000)
  • A. Degerman et al.

    Selective attention to sound location or pitch studied with fMRI

    Brain Res.

    (2006)
  • L.Y. Deouell et al.

    Cerebral responses to change in spatial location of unattended sounds

    Neuron

    (2007)
  • D.N. Gabriel et al.

    The eccentricity effect for auditory saccadic reaction times is independent of target frequency

    Hear. Res.

    (2010)
  • C.F. Geier et al.

    Circuitry underlying temporally extended spatial working memory

    Neuroimage

    (2007)
  • E. Gherri et al.

    Eye movement preparation causes spatially-specific modulation of auditory processing: new evidence from event-related brain potentials

    Brain Res.

    (2008)
  • M.A. Goodale et al.

    Separate visual pathways for perception and action

    Trends Neurosci.

    (1992)
  • J. Gottlieb

    Parietal mechanisms of target representation

    Curr. Opin. Neurobiol.

    (2002)
  • T.D. Griffiths

    Sensory systems: auditory action streams?

    Curr. Biol.

    (2008)
  • T.D. Griffiths et al.

    Human cortical areas selectively activated by apparent sound movement

    Curr. Biol.

    (1994)
  • T.D. Griffiths et al.

    Cortical activation during perception of a rotating wide-field acoustic stimulus

    Neuroimage

    (1999)
  • T.A. Hackett et al.

    Prefrontal connections of the parabelt auditory cortex in macaque monkeys

    Brain Res.

    (1999)
  • D.A. Hall

    Auditory pathways: are ‘what’ and ‘where’ appropriate?

    Curr. Biol.

    (2003)
  • R.S. Heffner et al.

    Sound-localization acuity and its relation to vision in large and small fruit-eating bats: I. Echolocating species, Phyllostomus hastatus and Carollia perspicillata

    Hear. Res.

    (2007)
  • G. Hickok et al.

    Towards a functional neuroanatomy of speech perception

    Trends Cogn. Sci.

    (2000)
  • G. Hickok et al.

    Dorsal and ventral streams: a framework for understanding aspects of the functional anatomy of language

    Cognition

    (2004)
  • M. Husain et al.

    Space and the parietal cortex

    Trends Cogn. Sci.

    (2007)
  • J. Ahveninen et al.

    Task-modulated “what” and “where” pathways in human auditory cortex

    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A.

    (2006)
  • C. Alain et al.

    “What” and “where” in the human auditory system

    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A.

    (2001)
  • C. Alain et al.

    The contribution of the inferior parietal lobe to auditory spatial working memory

    J. Cogn. Neurosci.

    (2008)
  • C. Alain et al.

    Dissociable memory- and response-related activity in parietal cortex during auditory spatial working memory

    Front. Aud. Cogn. Neurosci.

    (2010)
  • J.C. Alvarado et al.

    Cortex mediates multisensory but not unisensory integration in superior colliculus

    J. Neurosci.

    (2007)
  • R.A. Andersen

    Multimodal integration for the representation of space in the posterior parietal cortex

    Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci.

    (1997)
  • R.A. Andersen et al.

    Intentional maps in posterior parietal cortex

    Annu. Rev. Neurosci.

    (2002)
  • R.A. Andersen et al.

    Multimodal representation of space in the posterior parietal cortex and its use in planning movements

    Annu. Rev. Neurosci.

    (1997)
  • J.R. Angell

    Psychology: An Introductory Study of the Structure and Function of Human Consciousness

    (1908)
  • K.M. Armstrong et al.

    Selection and maintenance of spatial information by frontal eye field neurons

    J. Neurosci.

    (2009)
  • S.R. Arnott et al.

    Effects of perceptual context on event-related brain potentials during auditory spatial attention

    Psychophysiology

    (2002)
  • S.R. Arnott et al.

    The functional organization of auditory working memory as revealed by fMRI

    J. Cogn. Neurosci.

    (2005)
  • M. Bar et al.

    Top-down facilitation of visual recognition

    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A.

    (2006)
  • P. Belin et al.

    ‘What’, ‘where’ and ‘how’ in auditory cortex

    Nat. Neurosci.

    (2000)
  • J.W. Bisley et al.

    Neuronal activity in the lateral intraparietal area and spatial attention

    Science

    (2003)
  • G.J. Blatt et al.

    Visual receptive field organization and cortico-cortical connections of the lateral intraparietal area (area LIP) in the macaque

    J. Comp. Neurol.

    (1990)
  • M.R. Brown et al.

    Comparison of memory- and visually guided saccades using event-related fMRI

    J. Neurophysiol.

    (2004)
  • C.J. Bruce et al.

    Primate frontal eye fields. II. Physiological and anatomical correlates of electrically evoked eye movements

    J. Neurophysiol.

    (1985)
  • Cited by (0)

    View full text