Review
Threat-detection in child development: An evolutionary perspective

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.08.010Get rights and content

Abstract

Evidence for developmental aspects of fear-targets and anxiety suggests a complex but stable pattern whereby specific kinds of fears emerge at different periods of development. This developmental schedule seems appropriate to dangers encountered repeatedly during human evolution. Also consistent with evolutionary perspective, the threat-detection systems are domain-specific, comprising different kinds of cues to do with predation, intraspecific violence, contamination–contagion and status loss. Proper evolutionary models may also be relevant to outstanding issues in the domain, notably the connections between typical development and pathology.

Section snippets

Developmental schedule of fear-targets

A number of authors have noted that different domains of fear come online during human development. Stanley Hall's survey was perhaps the first systematic attempt to measure the prevalence of common fears on a large sample of children and adolescents, documenting the salience of natural triggers (thunder, reptiles, insects) as well as situational (darkness) and social ones (Hall, 1897).

Very broadly, one can delineate a general developmental schedule for typical childhood fears, starting with

Accounts of fear-acquisition

Central to early theories of fear-acquisition were classical conditioning accounts, following which any stimulus could become a fear-trigger, given the appropriate CS/UCS pairing (Watson and Rayner, 2000). The main assumption was that most normal fears as well as pathological phobias would be the result of actual particular encounters with the target stimuli. There were several problems with this account. First, the assumption of equipotentiality (all UCS have the same potential for triggering

Fear and precaution in an evolutionary perspective

The models reviewed above did not fully integrate evidence from ethological and evolutionary approaches to animal behavior. However, the processes engaged and in particular the process of acquisition may make more sense once considered as adaptive behaviors in the context of natural selection, which favors the evolution of domain-specific cognitive systems that handle recurrent fitness challenges. This domain-specific view of cognition informed by different principles was first popularized by

Stability and appropriateness of the developmental schedule

As noted above, children do not develop fear-targets in an unprincipled way. On the contrary, specific targets come on-line at different stages of childhood. This schedule is astonishingly stable between children in vastly different kinds of social and cultural environments, ranging from nomadic foraging tribes to large cities. Obviously, encounters with strange people, other animals, contaminants, differ a great deal between environments, so that children come to develop distinctly cultural

Potential vs. actual threats

It is striking that most children's fears occur in situations in which there is no actual direct threat – which may lead us to think that threat-detection systems are hyper-active or simply inaccurate in children. That would be misguided, for several reasons. First, as we emphasized, children's fears come online at times that correspond to actual dangers in evolutionary contexts. Second, as we will explain presently, children and adults are very similar in that respect, in the extent to which

Typical development and pathology

In our view, another advantage of the evolutionary perspective is to provide a clearer understanding of the similarities and differences between pathology and typical development. There is, as we mentioned in introduction, only a scant and scattered literature on the typical development of threat-detection. This is all the more striking in domains where children or adolescents develop pathologies, for which we often do not have very specific descriptions or theoretical models for typical

Outstanding questions

Several developmental issues remain to be addressed, of which the following are only a small selection.

  • Neurophysiological development. Threat-detection and precaution follow a strict developmental schedule. Although some structures supporting this cognitive domain are fairly well-identified (from neuro-imaging and neuro-psychological evidence, see Fiddick, this volume), there is still no general account of their unfolding during brain maturation. In some domains, like social competence and

References (126)

  • M. de Rosnay et al.

    Transmission of social anxiety from mother to infant: an experimental study using a social referencing paradigm

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (2006)
  • K.-A. Egliston et al.

    Inhibition of fear acquisition in toddlers following positive modelling by their mothers

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (2007)
  • L. Fiddick

    There is more than the amygdala: potential threat assessment in the cingulate cortex

    Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.

    (2011)
  • A.P. Field et al.

    Who's afraid of the big bad wolf: a prospective paradigm to test Rachman's indirect pathways in children

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (2001)
  • A.P. Field et al.

    Fear information and social phobic beliefs in children: a prospective paradigm and preliminary results

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (2003)
  • R. Franco et al.

    Environmental toxicity, oxidative stress and apoptosis: menage a trois

    Mutation Research

    (2009)
  • A.B.M. Gerdes et al.

    Spiders are special: fear and disgust evoked by pictures of arthropods

    Evolution and Human Behavior

    (2009)
  • J. Hahn-Holbrook et al.

    Parental precaution: neurobiological means and adaptive ends

    Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.

    (2011)
  • S.M. Korte et al.

    The Darwinian concept of stress: benefits of allostasis and costs of allostatic load and the trade-offs in health and disease

    Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews

    (2005)
  • H.L. Leonard et al.

    Childhood rituals: normal development or obsessive–compulsive symptoms?

    Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry

    (1990)
  • P. Lienard

    Life stages and risk-avoidance: status- and context-sensitivity in precaution systems

    Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.

    (2011)
  • I.M. Marks

    Innate and learned fears are at opposite ends of a continuum of associability

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (2002)
  • N. McNaughton et al.

    Theoretical approaches to the modeling of anxiety in animals

  • S. Mineka et al.

    Born to fear: non-associative vs associative factors in the etiology of phobias

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (2002)
  • P. Muris et al.

    Origins of common fears in South African children

    Journal of Anxiety Disorders

    (2008)
  • P. Muris et al.

    Common childhood fears and their origins

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (1997)
  • R.M. Nesse

    Proximate and evolutionary studies of anxiety, stress and depression: synergy at the interface

    Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews

    (1999)
  • S. Neuberg et al.

    Human Threat Management Systems: Self-Protection and Disease Avoidance

    Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.

    (2011)
  • R. Poulton et al.

    Evidence for a non-associative model of the acquisition of a fear of heights

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (1998)
  • R. Poulton et al.

    Non-associative fear acquisition: a review of the evidence from retrospective and longitudinal research

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (2002)
  • R.T. Abed et al.

    An evolutionary hypothesis for obsessive compulsive disorder: a psychological immune system?

    Behavioural Neurology

    (1998)
  • K.E. Adolph

    Specificity of learning: why infants fall over a veritable cliff

    Psychological Science

    (2000)
  • C.B. Bares et al.

    Knowledge of illness during childhood: making distinctions between cancer and colds

    International Journal of Behavioral Development

    (2008)
  • H.C. Barrett

    Adaptations to predators and prey

  • H.C. Barrett

    Enzymatic computation and cognitive modularity

    Mind & Language

    (2005)
  • C.J. Beale et al.

    What do our teenagers fear?

    Child Psychiatry Quarterly

    (1983)
  • Bergstrom, B., submitted for publication. Adaptive targets and chilhood...
  • L.L. Birch

    The control of food intake by young children: the role of learning

  • D.C. Blanchard et al.

    Human defensive behaviors to threat scenarios show parallels to fear- and anxiety-related defense patterns of non-human mammals

    Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews

    (2001)
  • I. Blanchette

    Snakes, spiders, guns, and syringes: how specific are evolutionary constraints on the detection of threatening stimuli?

    The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology

    (2006)
  • A.L. Bleske et al.

    Poaching, promiscuity, and deceit: combatting mating rivalry in same-sex friendships

    Personal Relationships

    (2001)
  • P. Boyer et al.

    Why ritualized behavior? Precaution systems and action parsing in developmental, pathological and cultural rituals

    Behavioral and Brain Sciences

    (2006)
  • P. Boyer et al.

    Ritual behavior in obsessive and normal individuals: moderating anxiety and reorganizing the flow of action

    Current Directions in Psychological Science

    (2008)
  • Boyer, P., Wilkie, L., submitted for publication. Memory for features of monsters and other...
  • J.J. Campos et al.

    Early experience and emotional development: the emergence of wariness of heights

    Psychological Science

    (1992)
  • A. Caramazza

    The interpretation of semantic category-specific deficits: what do they reveal about the organization of conceptual knowledge in the brain?

    Neurocase: Case Studies in Neuropsychology, Neuropsychiatry & Behavioural Neurology

    (1998)
  • L.J. Carver et al.

    The development of social information gathering in infancy: a model of neural substrates and developmental mechanisms

  • E. Cashdan

    A sensitive period for learning about food

    Human Nature

    (1994)
  • F. Clayton et al.

    Weaning age among foragers at Matjes river rock shelter, South Africa, from stable nitrogen and carbon isotope analyses

    American Journal of Physical Anthropology

    (2006)
  • C.M. Coelho et al.

    The origins of specific phobias: influential theories and current perspectives

    Review of General Psychology

    (2009)
  • Cited by (108)

    • Schizophrenia: The new etiological synthesis

      2022, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews
    • Enhanced contralateral theta oscillations and N170 amplitudes in occipitotemporal scalp regions underlie attentional bias to fearful faces

      2021, International Journal of Psychophysiology
      Citation Excerpt :

      Rapidly responding to threat-related stimuli has been necessary for survival in mammalian evolution and development (Boyer and Bergstrom, 2011; Lobue and Rakison, 2013).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text