Elsevier

Learning and Motivation

Volume 57, February 2017, Pages 48-60
Learning and Motivation

Working memory decline in normal aging: Is it really worse in space than in color?

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lmot.2017.01.007Get rights and content

Highlights

  • We compared allocentric spatial and color memory in young and older healthy adults.

  • We compared reference and working memory in young and older healthy adults.

  • Older adults performed worse than young adults on all memory tasks.

  • Some measures suggested a greater age-related decline for spatial working memory.

  • Most measures suggested a similar decline for spatial and color working memory.

Abstract

Aging is associated with a variety of changes in cognitive capacities, including a decline in working memory performance. Nevertheless, visuo-spatial working memory has been shown to exhibit a greater age-related decline than verbal working memory. Here, we assessed age-related changes in allocentric spatial working memory and color working memory. We tested 20–30-year-old and 65–75-year-old adults on four memory tasks requiring participants to learn, on a repeated-trial basis (i.e., reference memory) or a trial-unique basis (i.e., working memory), the locations or colors of three pads among 18 pads distributed in a real-world laboratory environment. Older adults performed worse than young adults on all memory tasks, but especially on working memory tasks. Some measures, including the older adults’ relative decrease in the number of correct choices before erring (CBE), as compared to young adults, and the number of trials with the first or first two choices correct, may suggest a greater age-related decline in allocentric spatial than color working memory. In contrast, the total number of disks visited to find the goals, the absolute decrease in CBE in older adults, the number of errorless trials and the number of trials with the first three choices correct revealed no age-related differences in working memory performance for spatial versus color information. We discuss how, depending on the measures used to evaluate memory performance, age-related declines in working memory may appear greater for spatial information because allocentric spatial memory may have quantitatively greater representational demands (i.e., require more bits of information) than color memory.

Introduction

Normal aging, in absence of evidence of mild cognitive impairment (MCI) or dementia, is associated with a variety of changes in cognitive capacities, one of which is an overall decline in working memory performance (Fabiani, 2012). Working memory refers to a brain system that enables temporary storage and manipulation of the information necessary for language comprehension, learning, and reasoning (Baddeley, 1992). Working memory can be evaluated by testing the ability of individuals to remember and use trial-unique information that must be distinguished from information acquired on previous trials, and is resistant to interference and distraction during the retention interval (Banta Lavenex, Colombo, Ribordy Lambert, & Lavenex, 2014; Bizon, Foster, Alexander, & Glisky, 2012; Spellman et al., 2015). Trial-unique procedures aimed at testing working memory are distinguished from repeated-trial procedures in which the information to be memorized remains constant over time, and which serve to evaluate what has been called reference memory (Banta Lavenex et al., 2014; Lee, Tucci, Sovrano, & Vallortigara, 2015; Morris, Hagan, & Rawlins, 1986; Olton, Becker, & Handelmann, 1979). In addition, information to be processed in working memory may be separated into distinct components including verbal, visual and spatial information, and, over the years, a number of experimental studies have reported evidence supporting the view that different types of information may be processed by different temporary storage systems (Baddeley, 2007, Baddeley, 2012, Logie, 2011, Logie and Marchetti, 1991).

Accordingly, a number of studies evaluated whether age-related declines in working memory performance differ based on the type of information to be remembered. In support of this view, it has been reported that visuo-spatial working memory exhibits a greater age-related decline than verbal working memory (Chen, Hale, & Myerson, 2003; Jenkins, Myerson, Joerding, & Hale, 2000; Myerson, Hale, Rhee, & Jenkins, 1999; Salthouse, 1995, Shelton et al., 1982). For example, Jenkins et al. (2000) tested young (18–24 years) and older (62–77 years) adults on a letter span task and a location span task presented on a computer screen. The letters were consonants, whereas the locations were represented by a 2.5 × 2.5 cm cross, presented in the center of one of sixteen cells of a 4 × 4 unfilled matrix (15 × 15 cm). In addition, participants performed these primary tasks, while sometimes performing a concurrent secondary task, including a verbal task or a visuo-spatial task. Older adults performed worse than young adults on all working memory tasks, irrespective of the type of material (verbal or spatial) or secondary task (none, verbal or spatial). However, Jenkins et al. (2000) reported a greater age-related performance difference in the memory span for locations (about 3 locations; 4.62 for older adults vs 7.58 for young adults) than in the memory span for letters (about 2 letters; 4.39 vs 6.46, respectively). These results were consistent with those of Myerson et al. (1999), who tested young (18–22 years) and older (63–69 years) adults on a digit span task and a location span task (with and without secondary tasks), and reported that older adults remembered on average one less digit (6.01 vs 6.98) and 2.4 fewer locations than young adults (3.96 vs 6.34). Interestingly, Chen et al. (2003) tested young (18–22 years) and older (65–75 years) adults on different object and location memory tasks. The object working memory tasks involved the presentation of shapes or textures, whereas the location working memory tasks involved the presentation of two dots defining a distance or the presentation of distinct textured dots defining specific locations. Although, in contrast to their previous studies (Jenkins et al., 2000, Myerson et al., 1999), Chen et al. (2003) did not find age-related differences in performance in the non-spatial working memory tasks, they again found that young adults outperformed older adults on the location tasks. Altogether, these results suggested that the age-related decline in working memory performance may differentially affect the maintenance of verbal, visual and spatial information (Myerson, Emery, White, & Hale, 2003).

It is important to note, however, that previous studies assessing visuo-spatial working memory in aging have utilised classical neuropsychological paradigms in which stimuli are presented on computer screens. They were thus limited to the assessment of egocentric spatial representations, which differ from the type of spatial representations an individual may build when moving about in a real-world environment. Indeed, the brain can represent locations via distinct spatial representation systems (Banta Lavenex and Lavenex, 2009, Burgess, 2006, O’Keefe and Nadel, 1978). Over the short term, and when an observer’s position is fixed in relation to an array of locations, egocentric (viewpoint-dependent, hippocampus-independent) coding is the most reliable, and allocentric (viewpoint-independent, hippocampus-dependent) encoding is unnecessary (Banta Lavenex & Lavenex, 2009). However, once the observer begins to move in the world, hippocampus-dependent allocentric spatial coding becomes critical to spatial memory processing (Banta Lavenex et al., 2014). It is therefore important to expand the investigation of age-related changes in working memory performance to compare the allocentric spatial working memory capacity of young and older adults. Moreover, whether the visuo-spatial component of working memory can and should be further subdivided into separable components, one for maintaining the visual features of the stimuli, such as shapes, textures or colors, and the other for maintaining their spatial locations, remains a matter of debate (Baddeley, 2012, Banta Lavenex et al., 2014, Chen et al., 2003). It is thus also important to compare potential age-related changes in allocentric spatial working memory with age-related changes in working memory in another visual domain, such as that for color.

Finally, previous studies of age-related changes in working memory performance did not compare the performance of the same participants on memory tasks where the same type of information can be acquired over repeated trials: i.e., on reference memory tasks (Banta Lavenex et al., 2014, Lee et al., 2015, Morris et al., 1986, Olton et al., 1979). Such comparison is important in order to shed light on the specific cognitive processes that may be particularly affected in normal aging, beyond working memory (Fabiani, 2012). Here, we tested whether there are: (1) different age-related declines in working memory, as compared to reference memory, for allocentric spatial and color information; and (2) greater age-related declines in allocentric spatial working memory than in color working memory.

Section snippets

Participants

Thirty-four young adults (16 males) aged 20–30 years (M = 24.15, SD = 3.46) and thirty-five older adults (18 males) aged 65–75 years (M = 69.26, SD = 3.02) took part in the study. Participants were recruited via personal connections, email postings on social networks, and via flyers distributed through local senior organisations. Care was taken to recruit participants from all education levels (Table 1). Exclusion criteria were subjective memory complaints, visual perception disabilities (i.e., color

Neuropsychological testing

All older adults were found to be within 1.75 standard deviations from the norm for age-matched controls for all the neuropsychological exams we performed (Table 1), including the Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) (Folstein et al., 1975); the Progressive Matrice-12 (Raven et al., 2003); the Vocabulary, Digit Span, Arithmetic and Similitude sub-tests from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-III (WAIS-III) (Wechsler, 1997); color vision with the Ishihara test (Ishihara, 1917); and the Corsi

Discussion

We found that older adults performed less well than young adults on all memory tasks, but especially on working memory tasks. On the one hand, some measures, including the relative number of correct choices before erring (CBE) and the number of trials with the first or first two choices correct, may suggest a greater age-related decline in allocentric spatial working memory than in color working memory. On the other hand, other measures, including the total number of disks visited to find the

Funding

This work was supported by Research Funds from the Faculty of Social and Political Sciences (SSP), University of Lausanne. The Faculty of SSP had no involvement in the study design, nor in the collection, analysis or interpretation of the data, or in the writing of this report or the decision to submit it for publication.

Declaration of interest

The authors declare no actual or potential conflict of interest that could inappropriately influence, or be perceived to influence, their work.

References (37)

  • J.L. Bizon et al.

    Characterizing cognitive aging of working memory and executive function in animal models

    Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience

    (2012)
  • S.G. Carmer et al.

    Evaluation of 10 pairwise multiple comparison procedures by Monte-Carlo methods

    Journal of the American Statistical Association

    (1973)
  • J. Chen et al.

    Effects of domain, retention interval, and information load on young and older adults’ visuospatial working memory

    Aging Neuropsychology and Cognition

    (2003)
  • P.M. Corsi

    Human memory and the medial temporal region of the brain

    Dissertation Abstracts International

    (1972)
  • F.I.M. Craik

    A functional account of age differenes in memory

  • M. D’Esposito

    From cognitive to neural models of working memory

    Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society London B Biological Sciences

    (2007)
  • M. Fabiani

    It was the best of times, it was the worst of times: A psychophysiologist's view of cognitive aging. [Research Support, N.I.H., Extramural Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t Review]

    Psychophysiology

    (2012)
  • S. Ishihara

    Tests for colour-blindness

    (1917)
  • Cited by (19)

    • Plastic changes to dendritic spines in the cerebellar and prefrontal cortices underlie the decline in motor coordination and working memory during successful aging

      2021, Behavioural Brain Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      Previous data suggests that aging may affect the speed at which novel information is acquired, thereby hindering cognitive processing [76]. This phenomenon is consistent with recent studies performed under different paradigms and that are applied to elderly rodents [39] and humans [20], as well as with the results of the present study related to vsWM. Accordingly, the decrease in dendritic spines observed is consistent with the behavioral findings unlike in previous reports where no differences in the density of spines in the basal arborization of these neurons were evident in old female rats [71].

    • Working memory decline in normal aging: Memory load and representational demands affect performance

      2017, Learning and Motivation
      Citation Excerpt :

      They were thus limited to the assessment of egocentric spatial representations, which differ from the allocentric spatial representations an individual may build when moving about in a real-world environment (Banta Lavenex & Lavenex, 2009; Burgess, 2006; O'Keefe & Nadel, 1978). To address this disparity, we recently expanded the investigation of age-related changes in working memory performance to compare age-related changes in allocentric spatial working memory and color working memory (Klencklen, Banta Lavenex, Brandner, & Lavenex, 2017). Specifically, we tested 20–30-year-old and 65–75-year-old adults on four memory tasks requiring participants to learn, on a repeated-trial basis (i.e., reference memory) or a trial-unique basis (i.e., working memory), the locations or colors of three pads among 18 pads distributed on the floor of a real-world laboratory environment.

    • Biology of cognitive aging across species

      2024, Geriatrics and Gerontology International
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text