The strength and generality of stimulus over-selectivity in simultaneous discrimination procedures

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lmot.2010.12.001Get rights and content

Abstract

Stimulus over-selectivity refers to behavior being controlled by one element of the environment at the expense of other equally salient aspects of the environment. This is a common problem for many individuals, including those with autism spectrum disorders, and learning difficulties, and presents a considerable problem for information processing in many important situations involving complex cues and environments. Three experiments explored the strength and generality of the over-selectivity effect in non-clinical adults undertaking a cognitively demanding task, by training and testing participants on a two-component trial-and-error discrimination learning task. The over-selectivity effect was found in a variety of test conditions, including when the comparison test stimulus was neutral (Experiment 1), novel (Experiment 2), neutral with no conditioning history (Experiment 3), or when punished during training (Experiments 1, 2 and 3). Such results provide ubiquity to the phenomenon, making it important to investigate further.

Research highlights

▶ We explore the strength and generality of the over-selectivity effect in non-clinical adults. ▶ Participants were trained and tested on a two-component discrimination learning task. ▶ Over-selectivity was found under many training/test conditions. ▶ Results provide ubiquity to the phenomenon of over-selectivity.

Section snippets

Experiment 1

As noted above, the typical procedure for investigating over-selectivity requires participants to choose between elements from previously reinforced and previously punished compound stimuli. Such discrimination performance reflects both a tendency to approach an element at test (the previously reinforced element), as well as a tendency to avoid an element (the previously punished stimulus). It is not clear whether, in this simultaneous discrimination procedure, over-selectivity between elements

Experiment 2

Experiment 2 aimed to replicate the over-selectivity effect using an alternative (non punished) test condition. The prior study reported here attempted to produce an associatively neutral stimulus. Experiment 2 extended this exploration by exploring the impact of employing a novel stimulus as the test comparator.

Experiment 3

Experiment 3, again, aimed to replicate the over-selectivity effect using a third alternative test condition. Re-examining the results of Experiment 1, in the light of Experiment 2, might suggest that the cues used in the former experiment were not really neutral, as a result of their conditioning history. It is, therefore, necessary to create elements with no associative strength and no conditioning history. If conditioning using a stimulus with no associative strength, but which is also not

General discussion

The present research aimed to investigate the relative strength and generality of the over-selectivity effect under a range of test conditions. Such research is important in order to begin understanding the potential theoretical framework of this effect. Previous work, regardless of the population, has typically focused on the standard method identified. This generates the possibility that the level of over-selectivity may be determined not only by learning about reinforced cues, but also by

Acknowledgment

Thanks are due to Bill Roberts for his helpful criticism and advice regarding the statistical analyses reported here.

References (32)

  • B. Birnie-Selwyn et al.

    Teaching children to spell: Decreasing consonant cluster errors by eliminating selective stimulus control

    Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis

    (1997)
  • R.J. Blanchard et al.

    Defensive reactions and exploratory behaviour in rats

    Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology

    (1974)
  • M.E. Carroll et al.

    Demonstrations of neophobia and enhanced neophobia in the albino rat

    Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology

    (1975)
  • W.V. Dube

    Stimulus overselectivity in discrimination learning

  • W.V. Dube et al.

    Eye tracking assessment of stimulus overselectivity in individuals with mental retardation

    Experimental Analysis of Human Behavior Bulletin

    (1999)
  • W.V. Dube et al.

    Reinforcer frequency and restricted stimulus control

    Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior

    (1997)
  • Cited by (18)

    • Over-selectivity decreases with increased training: A role for within-compound associations

      2019, Acta Psychologica
      Citation Excerpt :

      If the effect reduces over time (Azorlosa & Cicala, 1988; Reynolds & Reed, 2011b; Stout et al., 2003), then the difference in the extent to which the elements of the compound are judged to predict the illness should decrease as training continues. This would serve both to demonstrate an over-selectivity effect in a judgment paradigm of the used to explore human associative learning, and show that the over-selectivity effect reduces with training, which has been noted for rats in overshadowing procedures (Bellingham & Gillette, 1981; Stout et al., 2003), and in human simultaneous discrimination over-selectivity procedure (Reynolds & Reed, 2011a), but not in human judgment procedures. Fifteen participants (5 male and 10 female) aged between 19 and 25 years took part.

    • Unpredictability reduces over-selective responding of individuals with ASD who have language impairments

      2019, Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders
      Citation Excerpt :

      These data replicated the over-selectivity in Experiments 1 and 2 for the short-trained 100% groups (Kelly et al., 2016; Koegel & Wilhelm, 1973; Leader et al., 2009; Reed et al., 2009). Extending training reduced the over-selectivity effect, which has previously been noted for a nonclinical (Reynolds & Reed, 2011a) and ASD (Schover & Newsom, 1976; Schreibman et al., 1977) populations. That partial reinforcement did not greatly reduce over-selectivity suggests that the results reported by Koegel et al. (1979) with their 33% schedule might have more to do with the length of training employed.

    • Interference with facial emotion recognition by verbal but not visual loads

      2015, Research in Developmental Disabilities
      Citation Excerpt :

      Experiment 1 examined the effects of different concurrent tasks on the recognition of facial emotion in order to see if selectively interfering with one set of processes would differentially impact facial emotion recognition. To this end, a verbal counting task as employed as a verbal load in previous investigations (Andersson, Hagman, Talianzadeh, Svedberg, & Larsen, 2002; Reynolds & Reed, 2011), and a visual task previously employed by Reed and Gibson (2005) was also employed. Previous studies have demonstrated that concurrent tasks impact discrimination learning tasks (see Hanley, Pearson, & Young, 1990; Tracy & Robbins, 2008).

    • Event related potential analysis of stimulus over-selectivity

      2012, Research in Developmental Disabilities
      Citation Excerpt :

      The combination of stimulus elements that were reinforced were randomized, using the E-prime software, across participants to avoid any of the results being the product of some stimuli being intrinsically more salient than others. A concurrent load task was presented as previous reports have demonstrated that over-selectivity emerges in this population with such a task (see Reynolds & Reed, 2011). Participants were required to subtract seven continuously throughout the whole study, starting from a random five digit number (e.g., 76,654).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text