When young and older adults learn a map: The influence of individual visuo-spatial factors

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2016.12.002Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Young and older adults learned a map and performed a sketch map task.

  • Older adults were less accurate than young adults, in terms of missed locations.

  • The groups were equally accurate in terms of the landmarks placed on the map.

  • Visuo-spatial factors differently influence accuracy in the measures considered.

Abstract

The present study explores how age and individual visuo-spatial factors influence the processes underlying spatial mental representations derived by learning from a map. Forty young adults (24–35 years old) and 40 older adults (65–75 years old) were assessed on visuo-spatial abilities and self-assessed spatial preferences. Then they studied a map of a botanical garden and were asked to place a list of landmarks on a sketch. When missed locations were considered in calculating accuracy, older adults were less accurate than young adults, and accuracy was predicted by age and a preference for exploring new environments. When only the landmarks placed in the sketch were considered, however, older adults were as accurate as young adults, and accuracy was predicted only by visuo-spatial working memory. These findings show that spatial representations have features differently predicted by age and visuo-spatial factors, and are discussed in the light of aging and spatial neurocognitive models.

Introduction

Getting to know an environment is an experience typical of every stage of human life, from when children crawl to reach a toy under the chair to when the elderly go to their local pharmacy. The ability to remember the positions of objects or salient landmarks and how they relate to one another and to other features of the environment is associated with the formation of a mental map (Tolman, 1948), i.e., an internal representation of the layout of an environment that enables a flexible management of environmental information, such as landmarks and their relationships (Wolbers & Hegarty, 2010). Various factors are capable of modulating an individual's environment knowledge acquisition (Hegarty et al., 2006, Wolbers and Hegarty, 2010). Some of them are external, such as the type of input used to present spatial information (a map, verbal instructions, navigation); others are internal, and include age, i.e., young vs. older adulthood (differences have been identified between young adults in their twenties or thirties and adults in their sixties or more; Baltes & Staudinger, 2000), and individual visuo-spatial factors (such as visuo-spatial abilities and self-assessed spatial preferences). It is important to consider these factors, alone and in combination, when analyzing environment knowledge acquisition and how people's performance varies, even in old age (Shelton, Marchette, & Furman, 2013). The present study comes within this field of research and specifically focuses on: i) the map as a source of learning with the analysis of the underlying processes involved in mentally representing spatial information in young and older adults; and ii) how individual visuo-spatial factors support this mental representation.

Maps are commonly used to acquire spatial knowledge. They organize information allocentrically, showing landmarks and paths connecting them (as used in studies on young adults, e.g., Richardson et al., 1999, Thorndyke and Hayes-Roth, 1982). Maps reproduce large areas, such as a city or garden, on a small scale with respect to an individual's body and a single viewpoint is experienced. For these reasons, following Montello's (1993) definition, they can be considered as a figural space source, which differs from other media that are larger than the body and require the integration of information over time (as when navigating in environmental spaces). Studies use maps not only to assess people's ability to orient themselves in an environment (when, for instance, participants are shown a map and then asked to go along a path, e.g., Wilkniss, Jones, Korol, Gold, & Manning, 1997), but also to analyze the cognitive processes involved in their map learning and how landmarks are arranged on their mental maps (Borella et al., 2015, Coluccia, 2008, Coluccia et al., 2007). With aging, people become less able to develop such mental maps and neurocognitive models suggest that this is due to an age-related deterioration in the hippocampus. This is the area of the brain most involved both in a mental map's formation (Bird and Burgess, 2008, O'keefe and Nadel, 1978) – in the anterior part, and in its storage – in the posterior part (for a review, see Lithfous, Dufour, & Després, 2013).

Given the map-learning difficulties associated with aging, adopting an allocentric modality to present spatial information (e.g., showing a map) can prompt the formation of a cognitive map that poses fewer problems in terms of switching between views – to convert information from a path-following view to a configurational (allocentric) view, for instance. The literature indicates that the use of maps in aging studies thus seems to be less liable to age-related effects than when other types of input are used, such as navigation (e.g., Yamamoto & DeGirolamo, 2012), or spatial descriptions (e.g., Meneghetti, Borella, Grasso, & De Beni, 2012); age-related differences in map learning are influenced by multiple factors, however, such as the type of recall task used. Older adults nevertheless have more difficulty than young adults when asked to manage information learned from a map. In fact, after learning from a map, older adults perform less well than young adults when they need to imagine adopting different positions on the map (in pointing tasks), or when the format of their answer changes, such as when they answer questions about the spatial relations between landmarks (verification tests; e.g., Borella et al., 2015, Meneghetti et al., 2015).

Older adults may find things easier when the information encoded in a map is retrieved in the same format, i.e., when they are asked to freely draw a map of the environment (freehand map drawing task) or to position landmarks in a skeletal layout of the environment (sketch map task; Blades, 1990, Rovine and Weisman, 1989). In these types of task, the format (visuo-spatial) and the action (locating landmarks in relation to one another) match with the input used in the learning phase (the map). This avoids older people experiencing the cognitive overload that occurs when they are faced with other types of task and demand (e.g., Meneghetti, Borella, Gyselinck, & De Beni, 2012). Studies using graphical reproductions of environments (as in the freehand map drawing or sketch map tasks) have generated inconsistent results, however. There have been some reports of a similar performance in young and older adults (e.g., Meneghetti et al., 2012a, Yamamoto and DeGirolamo, 2012), while others found an impaired performance in older adults (e.g., Meneghetti et al., 2011, Meneghetti et al., 2015, Wilkniss et al., 1997). One of the reasons for these discrepancies could lie in the processes underlying spatial information recall (as demanded by different requests in a map drawing task). Indeed, spatial memory is not a unitary construct. We can distinguish between its function in landmark processing (i.e., object identity memory, Postma, Kessels, & Van Asselen, 2004), in exact, metric (Euclidean), coordinate processing (i.e., positional memory; McNamara, Hardy, & Hirtle, 1989), irrespective of the identity of an object, and its function in forming associations between the identity of objects and their positions (i.e., object-location memory; Kessels, Kappelle, de Haan, & Postma, 2002). Therefore landmark identity memory, positional memory and landmark-location binding represent different processes that can be highlighted when assessing the results of map drawing tasks. For instance, some studies considered map drawing accuracy in terms of the landmarks positioned correctly, as on a previously-learned map, noticing any landmarks that were wrongly positioned or omitted (e.g., Meneghetti et al., 2011, Moffat and Resnick, 2002). Other studies (based on a bidimensional regression method, Friedman & Kohler, 2003) explored to what degree the layout reproduced in the map drawing was distorted with respect to the original map learned (e.g., Yamamoto & DeGirolamo, 2012), and any missed locations were disregarded. Thus, considering landmark positioning with or without taking locations that are missing into account can measure different memory processes. In the former case, when missed locations are taken into account, the accuracy score is related more to memory, i.e., to the respondent's ability to remember positions. In the latter, when only the accurate positioning of the landmarks recalled is considered, the score can reflect the representation of the landmarks in relation to one another, i.e., the respondent's ability to associate the landmarks with their locations. Therefore, when we consider missed locations in judging accuracy, this may be a measure of positional memory, i.e., how many positions can be recalled, whereas if we consider only the landmarks placed on the map, then we are measuring accuracy in terms of object-location memory. These spatial memory processes rely on networks with different brain regions as major hubs, however: positional memory depends largely on the part of the hippocampus in the right hemisphere, while object-location memory relies on the hippocampus to some degree (mainly on the part in the left hemisphere) while other brain regions (such as the prefrontal cortex) are more strongly implicated (see Kessels, de Haan, Kappelle, & Postma, 2001 for a meta-analysis; Kessels, Hobbel, & Postma, 2007). In the light of the above assumptions, using different indices to measure recall accuracy in map recall tasks, with and without taking missed locations into account, can shed more light on participants' spatial representation capabilities (i.e., their positional and object-location memory), and thus on what is preserved or liable to decline in older adults.

Given that sketch map task can reveal different constructs of spatial memory, it is important to bear in mind other aspects capable of helping to explain how spatial learning accuracy can vary. One such aspect is the individual visuo-spatial factor, as emerged in studies on young adults (Hegarty et al., 2006, Weisberg et al., 2014). The term ‘visuo-spatial’ refers to a whole set of factors, including: the ability to retain and process visuo-spatial information (visuo-spatial working memory, VSWM); spatial cognitive skills, such as the ability to generate, retain and transform abstract visual images (Lohman, 1988), which is multi-faceted, like mental rotation and perspective-taking abilities; and also individual preferences and strategies employed to learn and represent an environment (as ascertained by means of visuo-spatial self-assessments). Studies have shown that, although VSWM and visuo-spatial abilities decline with aging (Borella et al., 2014, Techentin et al., 2014), they sustain the environment representations of both young and older adults (e.g., Meneghetti, Borella, Muffato, Pazzaglia, & De Beni, 2014). As concerns map learning, Borella et al. (2015) showed that: mental representations derived from map learning become extremely orientation-dependent with aging; and VSWM correlates with map learning accuracy – consistently with studies on young people showing a relationship between working memory (WM) and map learning (Coluccia, 2008, Coluccia et al., 2007) –. Map learning accuracy correlates with visuo-spatial (rotation) skills in young and older adults (as assessed using rotation tasks, e.g., Meneghetti et al., 2011), and these individual factors can mediate the relationship between age and environment knowledge (Kirasic, 2000, Meneghetti et al., 2014b). Meneghetti, Borella, Pastore, et al. (2014) showed, for instance, that visuo-spatial (rotation) abilities, VSWM and self-assessments mediate the relationship between age and the ability to orient oneself in an environment (using cardinal points). As introduced in the above-mentioned study, another individual factor to take into account concerns visuo-spatial preferences. Self-reported spatial cognitive style (Nori and Giusberti, 2003, Pazzaglia and Moè, 2013), sense of direction, spatial anxiety, and attitudes to novel environments all need to be considered in older people, as well as in young adults (e.g., Hegarty et al., 2006). Self-reported attitudes and preferences are relevant to performance in spatial tasks (Meneghetti et al., 2014a, Salthouse and Mitchell, 1990). There is evidence of older adults' sense of direction (De Beni et al., 2014, De Beni et al., 2006) correlating positively with their spatial learning (De Beni et al., 2014), even if a map is used as a source of information (De Beni et al., 2006). As for spatial anxiety, i.e., the self-reported level of worry when faced with spatial problems (such as having to find a certain place), it is demonstrated that this negatively affects spatial performance, in young adults at least (Lawton, 1994; for map learning: Thoresen et al., 2016), while its role when older adults learn from maps is less well known.

To sum up, administering map drawing task after participants have studied a map can offer insight on the underlying process of people's mental representations with aging. In particular, the first aim of the present study was to jointly consider two spatial memory constructs – positional and object-location memory – by considering landmark location accuracy in a map drawing task, both with and without taking any missing positions into account. We would expect to find age-related differences when missed locations were considered in the accuracy score (e.g., Meneghetti et al., 2011), but not when missed locations were disregarded (e.g., Yamamoto & DeGirolamo, 2012). We thus proposed to ascertain whether the two different spatial memory constructs are affected differently by aging. Other researchers have shown age-related differences in both positional and object-location memory (e.g., Kessels et al., 2007), but this issue has not been explored directly using the type of input and recall tasks method adopted here.

As a second aim, we analyzed the influence of individual visuo-spatial factors on map learning accuracy, assuming that VSWM, rotation abilities, and visuo-spatial preferences might be important (as suggested using other sources of learning; e.g., Meneghetti, Borella, Muffato, et al., 2014), albeit with differences in their degree of involvement depending on how accuracy was scored (with or without considering missed locations).

Section snippets

Participants

The study involved 80 participants: 40 young adults (aged 24 to 35; 19 females) and 40 older adults (aged 65 to 75; 19 females). All participants were volunteers. The older participants were all healthy and living independently, and they met our inclusion criterion requiring a score of > 27 in the Mini-Mental State Examination (Folstein, Folstein, & McHugh, 1975). The young adults had a higher education level than the older adults, F(1,79) = 16.53, η2 = 0.18, p < 0.001, consistently with the typical

Results

The statistical analyses were conducted using IBM SPSS Statistics 22 software.

Discussion and conclusion

The present study analyzed age-related differences in map learning accuracy using a sketch map task to assess young and older adult participants' mental representations. The first aim was to examine different aspects of their spatial representations, i.e., to distinguish between two different spatial memory constructs in aging, one related more to positional memory, the other to the representation of landmarks and their relative locations, by assessing accuracy in two ways, one with and one

References (58)

  • C. Meneghetti et al.

    Map learning in young and older adults: The influence of perceived stereotype threat

    Learning and Individual Differences

    (2015)
  • M.J. Rovine et al.

    Sketch-map variables as predictors of way-finding performance

    Journal of Environmental Psychology

    (1989)
  • A.L. Shelton et al.

    A mechanistic approach to individual differences in spatial learning, memory, and navigation

    The Psychology of Learning and Motivation

    (2013)
  • J.C. Thoresen et al.

    Not all anxious individuals get lost: Trait anxiety and mental rotation ability interact to explain performance in map-based route learning in men

    Neurobiology of Learning and Memory

    (2016)
  • P.W. Thorndyke et al.

    Differences in spatial knowledge acquired from maps and navigation

    Cognitive Psychology

    (1982)
  • T. Wolbers et al.

    What determines our navigational abilities?

    Trends in Cognitive Sciences

    (2010)
  • P.B. Baltes et al.

    Wisdom: A metaheuristic (pragmatic) to orchestrate mind and virtue toward excellence

    American Psychologist

    (2000)
  • C.M. Bird et al.

    The hippocampus and memory: Insights from spatial processing

    Nature Reviews Neuroscience

    (2008)
  • E. Borella et al.

    Spatial abilities across the adult life span

    Developmental Psychology

    (2014)
  • E. Borella et al.

    Map learning and the alignment effect in young and older adults: How do they gain from having a map available while performing pointing tasks?

    Psychological Research

    (2015)
  • E. Coluccia

    Learning from maps: The role of visuo-spatial working memory

    Applied Cognitive Psychology

    (2008)
  • E. Coluccia et al.

    The role of visuo-spatial working memory in map learning: New findings from a map drawing paradigm

    Psychological Research

    (2007)
  • F.I.M. Craik et al.

    Handbook of aging and cognition

    (2008)
  • R. De Beni et al.

    The role of mental rotation and age in spatial perspective-taking tasks: When age does not impair perspective-taking performance

    Applied Cognitive Psychology

    (2006)
  • R. De Beni et al.

    BAC. Portfolio per la valutazione del benessere e delle abilità cognitive nell'età adulta e avanzata

    (2008)
  • R. De Beni et al.

    Batteria visuo-spaziale. Strumenti per la valutazione delle abilità visuo-spaziali nell'arco di vita adulta

    (2014)
  • A. Friedman et al.

    Bidimensional regression: Assessing the configural similarity and accuracy of cognitive maps and other two-dimensional data sets

    Psychological Methods

    (2003)
  • A.L. Gardony et al.

    Gardony map drawing analyzer: Software for quantitative analysis of sketch maps

    Behavior Research Methods

    (2015)
  • ISTAT

    Annuario statistico italiano 2011 (Italian statistics yearbook 2011)

    (2011)
  • Cited by (17)

    • The effect of sound environment on spatial knowledge acquisition in a virtual outpatient polyclinic

      2022, Applied Ergonomics
      Citation Excerpt :

      In this task, the participants were presented with a schematic plan of the outpatient polyclinic that showed the beginning of the route. They were asked to place the escalator, the staircases, the elevators, and the patient administration desks on the blank plan as accurately as possible similar to previous studies (Meneghetti et al., 2017; Muffato et al., 2017). For scoring purposes, the completed sketch maps were scanned and uploaded each plan to Gardony Map Drawing Analyzer (GMDA version 1.2) (Gardony et al., 2016).

    • Self-evaluation by community older adults on the applicability of the healthy beat acupunch exercise program

      2019, Complementary Therapies in Medicine
      Citation Excerpt :

      Confidence seems to have an impact on older adults’ perceptions towards exercise motion difficulty 11 Nevertheless, the older adults in this study still agreed that the motions offered benefits and value for their health. Older adults are usually interested in learning new skills with challenges, 20 although they have certain difficulties in learning new skills. 8,21,22 A sense of achievement is an important motivator for increasing older adults’ adherence to an exercise.

    • Geometrical distortions in geographical cognitive maps

      2018, Journal of Environmental Psychology
      Citation Excerpt :

      Similarly, Walmsley and Jenkins (1992) showed that drivers develop cognitive maps of new areas quicker than non-drivers, and that people from large cities draw more detailed maps than people from small towns and rural areas. Furthermore, the effect of aging on sketch map accuracy has been investigated by Muffato, Meneghetti, Di Ruocco, and De Beni (2017), showing a general reduced accuracy in sketches drawn by elderly participants. This paper aimed to contribute to a better formalization of geometrical distortions in geographical large-scale cognitive maps.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text