Individual differences in cognitive resources and elementary school mathematics achievement: Examining the roles of storage and attention

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2014.11.008Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Short-term memory predicted differences in math performance from preschool through grade 5.

  • A parallel finding emerged with a measure of attention based on cognitive assessment in preschool.

  • Attention measured by parental report did not predict math achievement in preschool or over time.

  • Early cognitive assessment may identify students at risk for long-term underachievement in math.

Abstract

Using data from the NICHD Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development, a longitudinal study of 1,364 children, the present research investigated early cognitive correlates of children's mathematics achievement. Individual differences in mathematics performance prior to kindergarten and growth in achievement across elementary school were modeled as a function of two measures of attention—performance-based and informant-reported—and a measure of short-term memory. At the age of 54 months, children with poor capacities for attention and short-term memory storage had lower mathematics achievement than their peers with larger capacities. These early differences persisted through the fifth grade. Interestingly, the informant-based measure of attention did not predict mathematics achievement or its growth across time. The findings implicate the utility of early performance-based assessments of cognitive resources – storage and attention – in identifying students potentially at-risk for poor mathematical outcomes.

Introduction

In carrying out our daily tasks we depend on cognitive resources that allow us to hold and manipulate information in mind in order to reach a goal. For example, tasks as diverse as preparing a dinner or solving a mathematics problem involve processing information, retrieved either from an external source (e.g., a book) or from long-term memory. This, in turn, requires a person to temporarily store relevant information and manipulate it, all while resisting potentially distracting stimuli. Thus, both storage capacity and attentional resources are critical for human behavior and, as reviewed below, they have been shown to play a key role in learning. Yet, a more nuanced understanding of how specific cognitive resources are linked to growth in children's performance is required in order to design tools to identify students at risk for poor academic achievement and to create interventions targeting these students. To this end, the current paper presents the results of a longitudinal investigation that seeks to better understand the relation between children's cognitive resources—storage and attention—assessed prior to school entry and their academic achievement throughout elementary school. We focus specifically on mathematics achievement because of the foundational role early mathematical performance has been shown to play in students' long-term academic outcomes (Duncan et al., 2007) and career aspirations (Mullis et al., 2012, National Center for Education Statistics, 2011).

The processes of storing information and allocating attention have often been examined within the domain of research on working memory. There are various theoretical accounts of working memory (Baddeley, 2000, Baddeley and Hitch, 1974, Baddeley and Hitch, 1994, Bayliss et al., 2003, Engle et al., 1999, Jarrold et al., 2014). Whereas the original Baddeley and Hitch (1974) model has been subsequently extended and revised, most scholars agree with the original notion that two of the key resources required for the functioning of working memory are the capacity for temporary storage and controlled attention; these resources work together to enable recalling and manipulating information (Miyake & Shah, 1999). Storage involves mental coding of incoming information, or short-term memory (Baddeley and Hitch, 1974, Baddeley and Hitch, 1994). While short-term memory requires some attention in order to rehearse stored information (Cowan, 2001), working memory requires a greater level of attention for manipulation of that information.

Multiple studies have documented a relation between working memory and academic performance, both for overall academic achievement and for mathematics achievement in particular (Adams and Hitch, 1997, Alloway and Alloway, 2010, Alloway and Passolunghi, 2011, Andersson, 2007, Bull and Scerif, 2001, De Smedt et al., 2009, Holmes et al., 2008, Lee et al., 2009). Limitations in working memory have been identified as one of the key cognitive factors (along with domain-specific factors, such as poor underlying numerical representations) linked to general learning disabilities (Alloway et al., 2009, Swanson and Ashbaker, 2000, Tillman et al., 2011) and to mathematical learning disabilities (Geary et al., 2012, Hitch and McAuley, 1991, McLean and Hitch, 1999, Pickering and Gathercole, 2004, Swanson and Jerman, 2006).

Although there is a large body of evidence indicating a relation between working memory and mathematics performance, researchers have highlighted the importance of understanding the underlying nature of this relation (see Raghubar, Barnes, & Hecht, 2010 for a review). The idea that working memory taps multiple cognitive resources suggests that a deficit in working memory may be due to one or more deficits in these resources. Cowan and Alloway (2009) note that working memory theorists have long been divided in their views on the source of forgetting, debating whether the main source is the control of attention (Engle et al., 1999) or limitations in storage capacity and the rate of storage decay (Baddeley, 1986, Broadbent, 1958, Cowan, 1999, Miller, 1958). Further, there may be differences among individuals in the distribution of cognitive resources (i.e., storage and attention) utilized by the working memory system. For example, some students may have a limited capacity for short-term storage but a strong ability to control attention; the academic difficulties of these students would be driven specifically by this storage limitation, rather than a failure of attention. Conversely, other students might show deficits in working memory performance and academic achievement because of difficulties controlling attention, even if those students have a large capacity for storing information. Achieving greater clarity concerning the distinct contributions of storage and regulatory resources may be instrumental in our ability to design effective interventions that directly target the sources of a child's difficulties.

A number of studies have examined either attention or short-term memory in relation to children's mathematical achievement. For example, meta-analytic findings have shown that the capacity for attention in preschool and kindergarten predicts mathematical performance later in elementary school (Duncan et al., 2007). Similarly, young children's short-term memory is related to their current and later mathematics achievement (Alloway and Passolunghi, 2011, Andersson, 2010, De Smedt et al., 2009, Purpura and Ganley, 2014, Swanson and Kim, 2007). However, researchers have become increasingly aware of the need to measure both attention and storage components contemporaneously when predicting children's academic performance. This trend is reflected in a series of recent longitudinal investigations of elementary school children's mathematics achievement, which are described below. These studies provide compelling evidence that early working memory could serve as a window into children's mathematics achievement trajectories, by disentangling a number of competing factors. Yet, the findings concerning a specific role of attention are somewhat inconsistent across the studies.

An investigation conducted by Fuchs et al. (2010) with first graders measured attention using teacher ratings on the Strengths and Weaknesses of ADHD-Symptoms and Normal-Behavior (SWAN) scale (Swanson et al., 2004), and measured working memory and numerical knowledge through direct assessment of test performance. The findings showed that attention scores predicted some, but not all of the measures of mathematics performance, after controlling for working memory, which was also a significant predictor. However, in their discussion of these findings, the authors note that their informant-based measure of attention might have been a proxy for academic achievement, as teachers' ratings could have been biased by their knowledge of children's academic performance. Similar results were obtained in a comprehensive longitudinal investigation of growth in children's mathematical ability from kindergarten through grade 5, although the measure of working memory was taken during elementary school (Geary et al., 2012).

In contrast, Swanson (2011) conducted a similar investigation but obtained different results. Growth in children's word problem solving performance was examined from grades 1 through 3, as a function of their working memory, numerical knowledge, phonological processing, and teacher reports of inattentive behavior on the Conners' Teacher Rating Scale-Revised form (Conners, 1997). Working memory predicted growth and grade 3 levels of mathematics performance, but inattentive behavior was not found to be a significant predictor of problem solving performance in grade 3 after accounting for working memory.

To summarize, several existing studies have examined attention separately from working memory measures and have implicated both attention and storage in mathematics performance. Yet, the findings are not entirely consistent, which may be due, in part, to the measures of attention used in these investigations. In particular, a separate measure of attention was based on teacher reports of inattentive behavior. As the investigators have pointed out themselves, the informant-based reports of inattentive behavior may have been a proxy for other variables, such as academic achievement (Purpura & Lonigan, 2009). Performance-based assessment of attention may provide a more accurate measure whereas measures based on informant report might neglect attention deficits in students without behavior problems and thus, may fail to capture the true variability among students (Jaekel, Wolke, & Bartmann, 2013).

Some of these studies have also captured attention via measures of the central executive, which is the component of working memory that is responsible for manipulation of information through controlled attention and inhibition of irrelevant stimuli (Baddeley & Hitch, 1974). The use of instruments testing the central executive component as a measure of attention also presents problems with interpretation, because these instruments typically involve a large storage component in addition to an attention-regulating component, making it hard to ascertain the distinct roles of different cognitive resources (Gathercole and Pickering, 2000, Szmalec et al., 2005). For example, a measure of the central executive commonly used with children is the listening span task (e.g., De Weerdt et al., 2013, Gathercole and Pickering, 2000, Pickering and Gathercole, 2004, Geary et al., 2012). In this task, children are presented with a series of statements that they must identify as either true or false; at the end of the series, they are asked to recall the last word of each sentence, which clearly requires both storage and attention resources. In order to better understand the distinctive contribution of attention, it would be useful to use tasks with a minimal storage requirement.

Further, in examining children's capacity for storage and attention, it is important to measure them early, preferably even before kindergarten entry. This can help educators identify children who may be at risk for poor mathematics achievement and provide them with additional training and monitoring. Yet, existing studies have primarily examined these cognitive resources in school-aged children, although some have examined either short-term memory, attention, or a composite of executive function in younger children (Clark et al., 2010, Fuhs et al., 2014, Purpura and Ganley, 2014). Moreover, even though several investigations have taken a longitudinal approach, some of them looked at concurrent relations (e.g., Fuchs et al., 2010), and others (e.g., Geary et al., 2012) have used measures of attention and the central executive that were averaged over time. This leaves open the question of whether cognitive resources evaluated prior to school entry can be used to improve children's preparedness for mathematical learning in school.

The overarching question of this research was: to what extent do early measures of attention and short-term storage predict differences in elementary school students' mathematics achievement? In addressing this question, we examined early correlates of children's mathematics achievement before school entry and the nature of growth in mathematics achievement over time. In both cases, we focused on children's capacities for storage and attention prior to kindergarten and examined their distinct contributions in predicting mathematics achievement. Importantly, we included two types of measures of attention—one based on informant report and the other based on child assessment. This allowed us to determine whether either or both types of attention measures would carry any predictive weight after partitioning variance in mathematics achievement that is explained by short-term storage.

Section snippets

Participants

The data used in the present investigation were drawn from a larger NICHD Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development (SECCYD; NICHD Early Child Care Research Network, 1994), a longitudinal study of children and their families. As part of the SECCYD, the families of full-term, healthy newborns were recruited through hospitals across the United States in 1991. The study website, https://secc.rti.org, contains further details about recruitment and consent procedures.

The sample consisted of

Descriptive analysis

Table 1 provides means and standard deviations and Table 2 provides bivariate correlations for all continuous variables used in our analyses. As shown in Table 2, correlations between predictors and covariates were small (< .10–.30), whereas correlations between predictors/covariates and mathematics achievement were moderate to strong (.33–.65), based on effect size conventions (Cohen, 1988). Small but significant correlations were also seen between the three predictors, with higher

Discussion

There is a large body of literature establishing the link between working memory and academic performance. In conceptualizing working memory, it is generally agreed that its functioning draws on two kinds of cognitive resources—those involved in storing information and those involved in regulating attention. Few studies, however, have simultaneously examined storage and attention as distinct contributors to students' performance. Furthermore, these studies have mainly relied on informant

Conclusions

There is a substantial amount of evidence linking working memory to mathematical performance in children. Yet, two cognitive resources theorized to be central to the working memory system, short-term storage of information and regulation of attention, have rarely been measured separately in studies of children's mathematics achievement. The small body of work that has captured this distinction has yielded inconsistent findings and has mainly relied on informant reports of inattentive behavior.

References (72)

  • S.C. Levine et al.

    Development of calculation abilities in young children

    Journal of Experimental Child Psychology

    (1992)
  • J.F. McLean et al.

    Working memory impairments in children with specific arithmetic learning difficulties

    Journal of Experimental Child Psychology

    (1999)
  • D.J. Purpura et al.

    Working memory and language: Skill-specific or domain-general relations to mathematics?

    Journal of Experimental Child Psychology

    (2014)
  • K.P. Raghubar et al.

    Working memory and mathematics: A review of developmental, individual difference, and cognitive approaches

    Learning and Individual Differences

    (2010)
  • P. Starkey

    The early development of numerical reasoning

    Cognition

    (1992)
  • H.L. Swanson et al.

    Working memory, short-term memory, speech rate, word recognition and reading comprehension in learning disabled readers: Does the executive system have a role?

    Intelligence

    (2000)
  • L. Swanson et al.

    Working memory, short-term memory, and naming speed as predictors of children's mathematical performance

    Intelligence

    (2007)
  • H. Aase et al.

    Infrequent, but not frequent, reinforcers produce more variable responding and deficient sustained attention in young children with Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD)

    Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry

    (2006)
  • T.M. Achenbach

    Manual for the child behavior checklist/4–18 and profile

    (1991)
  • T.P. Alloway et al.

    The cognitive and behavioral characteristics of children with low working memory

    Child Development

    (2009)
  • T.P. Alloway et al.

    Verbal and visuo-spatial short-term and working memory in children: Are they separable?

    Child Development

    (2006)
  • U. Andersson

    The contribution of working memory to children's mathematical word problem solving

    Applied Cognitive Psychology

    (2007)
  • U. Andersson

    Skill development in different components of arithmetic and basic cognitive functions: Findings from a 3-year longitudinal study of children with different types of learning difficulties

    Journal of Educational Psychology

    (2010)
  • A. Baddeley

    Working memory

    (1986)
  • A.D. Baddeley et al.

    Working memory

  • A.D. Baddeley et al.

    Developments in the concept of working memory

    Neuropsychology

    (1994)
  • R.A. Barkley

    Behavioural inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD

    Psychological Bulletin

    (1997)
  • D.M. Bayliss et al.

    The complexities of complex span: explaining individual differences in working memory in children and adults

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: General

    (2003)
  • D.E. Broadbent

    Perception and communication

    (1958)
  • R. Bull et al.

    Executive functioning as a predictor of children's mathematics ability: Inhibition, switching, and working memory

    Developmental Neuropsychology

    (2001)
  • C.A.C. Clark et al.

    Preschool executive functioning abilities predict early mathematics achievement

    Developmental Psychology

    (2010)
  • J. Cohen

    Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences

    (1988)
  • C.K. Conners

    Conners' rating scales — Revised: Long form

    (1997)
  • N. Cowan

    An embedded-processes model of working memory

  • N. Cowan

    The magical number 4 in short-term memory: A reconsideration of mental storage capacity

    Behavioral and Brain Sciences

    (2001)
  • N. Cowan et al.

    Development of working memory in childhood

    (2009)
  • Cited by (15)

    • Intelligence quotient testing in children with hearing loss: A systematic review

      2024, American Journal of Otolaryngology - Head and Neck Medicine and Surgery
    • Longitudinal relations between self-regulatory skills and mathematics achievement in early elementary school children from Chinese American immigrant families

      2023, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology
      Citation Excerpt :

      This is because they capture relatively pure information about one’s self-regulatory capacities in an optimal setting (Gerst et al., 2017; Toplak et al., 2013). Indeed, researchers have found positive associations between performance on math achievement tests and lab-based cognitive measures of attention focusing (e.g., Anobile et al., 2013; Commodari & Di Blasi, 2014; Dulaney et al., 2015) and inhibitory control (e.g., Agostino et al., 2010; Blair & Razza, 2007; Fuhs et al., 2015; Monette et al., 2011; Verdine et al., 2014) in preschool- to school-aged children. Behavioral measures of self-regulation tapping multiple self-regulatory skills have likewise shown positive associations with math achievement.

    • Domain-specific effects of attentional and behavioral control in early literacy and numeracy development

      2018, Learning and Individual Differences
      Citation Excerpt :

      This latter relationship was expected to be partly indirect, via phonological awareness. Furthermore, considering the previously found robust relations between EF and mathematics (Allan et al., 2014; Blair & Razza, 2007; Dulaney et al., 2015; Geary, 2011), we expected both attentional and behavioral control to directly predict number sense in kindergarten and mathematics in first grade. Ninety children (48 boys and 42 girls) were recruited from three public schools in three different cities in the south of the Netherlands.

    • From classroom environment to mathematics achievement: The mediating role of self-perceived ability and subject interest

      2016, Learning and Individual Differences
      Citation Excerpt :

      To some extent, people differ in mathematics achievement because they vary in abilities that are important for learning mathematics. For example, individual differences in maths performance have been found to be associated with individual differences in memory (Swanson & Sachse-Lee, 2001), processing speed (Geary, 2011; Taub, Keith, Floyd, & McGrew, 2008), intelligence (Deary, Strand, Smith, & Fernandes, 2007), attention (Dulaney, Vasilyeva, & O'Dwyer, 2015), language ability (Vukovic & Lesaux, 2013) and spatial skills (Rohde & Thompson, 2007; Tosto et al., 2014). A robust body of research further suggests that individual differences in motivation are also associated with maths achievement (Elliot & Dweck, 2005; Robbins et al., 2004).

    • How children learn math: The science of math learning in research and practice

      2022, How Children Learn Math: The Science of Math Learning in Research and Practice
    View all citing articles on Scopus

    The first author was the data analyst for the present study and accessed the NICHD SECCYD data in collaboration with Dr. Eric Dearing, through a security agreement with the Research Triangle Institute. Special thanks to Eric for his invaluable support during the initial stages of this project; our earlier collaboration provided the foundation for this work.

    View full text