Differential contribution of specific working memory components to mathematics achievement in 2nd and 3rd graders

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2009.08.004Get rights and content

Abstract

The contribution of the three core components of working memory (WM) to the development of mathematical skills in young children is poorly understood. The relation between specific WM components and Numerical Operations, which emphasize computation and fact retrieval, and Mathematical Reasoning, which emphasizes verbal problem solving abilities in 48 2nd and 50 3rd graders was assessed using standardized WM and mathematical achievement measures. For 2nd graders, the central executive and phonological components predicted Mathematical Reasoning skills; whereas the visuo-spatial component predicted both Mathematical Reasoning and Numerical Operations skills in 3rd graders. This pattern suggests that the central executive and phonological loop facilitate performance during early stages of mathematical learning whereas visuo-spatial representations play an increasingly important role during later stages. We propose that these changes reflect a shift from prefrontal to parietal cortical functions during mathematical skill acquisition. Implications for learning and individual differences are discussed.

Introduction

Although the basics of mathematics are among the more important competencies that children need to master for successful living in modern societies, our understanding of the cognitive mechanisms that support mathematics learning is limited (Mazzocco, 2008). What is known suggests that working memory (WM) is pivotal to many aspects of learning mathematics (Bull et al., 2008, Geary, 1990, Geary and Brown, 1991, Geary et al., 2000, Geary et al., 2004, Hitch and McAuley, 1991, Passolunghi and Siegel, 2001, Passolunghi and Siegel, 2004, Siegel and Ryan, 1989, Swanson, 1993, Swanson, 1994, Swanson and Sachse-Lee, 2001, van der Sluis et al., 2005, Wilson and Swanson, 2001). However, the relations between the different components of WM and mathematical competence are not as well established in children compared to adults (Ashcraft, 1992, Furst and Hitch, 2000, Hecht, 2002, Lemaire et al., 1996, Logie et al., 1994).

Many children begin school with an implicit understanding of aspects of number, counting, and arithmetic and WM may contribute to their ability to build on this informal knowledge during schooling (Geary and Brown, 1991, Geary and Burlingham-Dubree, 1989, Siegler and Jenkins, 1989). Children who excel in early mathematics learning tend to have high WM capacity (Hoard et al., 2008, Passolunghi et al., 2008), and mathematically gifted adolescents tend to have enhanced visuo-spatial WM (Dark & Benbow, 1990). WM has also been reported to mediate the relationship between IQ and mathematical performance as early as the 1st grade (Passolunghi et al., 2008). Children's learning of the mathematical number line is influenced by a combination of intelligence, the central executive, and visuo-spatial WM. However, the relative contributions of these WM components to learning changes from 1st to 2nd grade as the central executive increases in importance, whereas the roles of intelligence and visuo-spatial WM decline (Geary, Hoard, Nugent & Byrd-Craven, 2008). In short, we know that WM is critical for mathematics learning but we do not fully understand how the different components of WM contribute to learning in different areas of mathematics and we do not know whether the importance of one or more WM components changes as learning progresses. We begin with a brief review of the components of WM and their relation to mathematics learning, and then outline how the current study addresses the issue of whether the relative importance of the different WM systems changes from one grade to the next.

WM is a cognitive system specialized for storage and manipulation of information (Baddeley, Hitch & Bower, 1974). Although different theoretical models of WM have been proposed (for a review see Miyake & Shah, 1999), Baddeley and Hitch's model has been the most influential. In this model, WM is composed of a central executive and two subsystems for temporary storage and rehearsal of auditory-verbal and visuo-spatial information, the phonological loop and the visuo-spatial sketchpad, respectively (Baddeley, 1986, Baddeley, 1996, Baddeley et al., 1974, Miyake and Shah, 1999).

The central executive plays an important role in sequencing operations, coordinating the flow of information, and guiding decision-making (Baddeley, 1996, Baddeley et al., 1998), particularly when problems are more complex and facts cannot be easily retrieved from memory. The central executive is important for many aspects of mathematical performance, including use of complex arithmetic procedures that involve carrying and borrowing operations (Ashcraft, 1992, De Rammelaere et al., 1999, De Rammelaere et al., 2001, Frensch and Geary, 1993, Geary et al., 1993, Hecht, 2002, Lemaire et al., 1996). The two other components of WM are specialized for storage of domain-specific information. The phonological loop is involved in encoding and maintaining arithmetical operands (Furst and Hitch, 2000, Logie et al., 1994, Noel et al., 2001) and maintaining intermediate results (Heathcote, 1994), but not specifically in calculation of answers (De Rammelaere et al., 1999, Furst and Hitch, 2000, Hecht, 2002, Lemaire et al., 1996). Furst and Hitch showed that the phonological loop is involved in retaining and storing information about complex problems, but it is not critically involved in retrieving factual mathematical knowledge (Furst & Hitch, 2000). Consistent with this, the relationship between phonological loop and adults' mathematical performance has been relatively weak, except in dual-task paradigms when the phonological loop is excessively taxed (Heathcote, 1994, Lehto, 1995, Logie and Baddeley, 1987, Logie et al., 1994). In adults, the visuo-spatial sketchpad has been implicated in solving multi-digit operations (Heathcote, 1994) and in more complex algebraic and geometric problem solving (Reuhkala, 2001). Notwithstanding these findings, the role of the visuo-spatial sketchpad in mathematical cognition remains poorly understood.

Each WM component has a specialized role in mathematical cognition that varies with expertise and development. Different levels of experience with numbers and mathematical concepts, familiarity of the stimuli and the strength of representations can lead to changes in the types of strategies applied to solve a mathematical task; this in turn calls upon different WM components (Gathercole and Adams, 1994, Henry and Miller, 1991).

Children under the age of seven tend to rely more on visual memory to remember material such as pictures of familiar and nameable objects rather than coding visual items to verbal labels (Hitch, Halliday, Schaafstal & Schraagen, 1988). Some researchers have suggested that preschoolers tend to perform better on nonverbal rather than verbal arithmetic tasks and that the visuo-spatial sketchpad capacity is the best predictor of these abilities in this age group (Levine et al., 1992, McKenzie et al., 2003, Rasmussen and Bisanz, 2005, Simmons et al., 2008). From the age of seven onwards, however, children increasingly rely on verbal rehearsal to maintain information in memory, thus recruiting the phonological loop (Hitch et al., 1988). Consistent with this, Rasmussen and Bisanz found that by the 1st grade, performance becomes equivalent on nonverbal and verbal mathematical tasks, and that the phonological loop becomes the best predictor of performance on verbal mathematics problems (Rasmussen & Bisanz, 2005). WM also influences math performance in elementary school: in a large sample of 1st, 2nd and 3rd graders, Swanson found that younger children and children who were poor mathematical problem solvers performed less well on WM tasks than older children or children who were good problem solvers (Swanson & Beebe-Frankenberger, 2004). However, the specific contributions of each WM component across grades were not examined.

In 7- to 8-year-old children, one study found that mathematics performance is most strongly correlated with the central executive, followed by the phonological loop (L. Henry & MacLean, 2003). As the supervisory system, the central executive facilitates children's problem solving by aiding in selection of appropriate strategies (Barrouillet and Lepine, 2005, Bull et al., 1999, Geary et al., 2004) and by allocating attention resources to implement the strategy execution. Using a longitudinal design, Gathercole and Pickering found that central executive measures shared significant and unique links with children's standardized test scores in mental arithmetic at 7 years of age and again at 8 years of age (Gathercole & Pickering, 2000). On the other hand, in a large sample of 8- to 11-year-old children, Adams and Hitch found that articulatory suppression significantly disrupted children's ability to solve arithmetic problems (Adams, Hitch & Donlan, 1998), suggesting an important role for the phonological loop. Other studies have suggested that the phonological loop is engaged when children transform symbol and number strings into verbal code when using verbally mediated counting strategies during basic arithmetic problem solving (Baddeley and Logie, 1987, Geary et al., 1996, Geary et al., 1993, Logie et al., 1994, Miura et al., 1999). More recently, Holmes and Adams found that in a group of typically developing 8- and 9-year-olds, the central executive and the visuo-spatial sketchpad, but not the phonological loop scores, predicted overall curriculum-based mathematics achievement (Holmes & Adams, 2006). Interestingly, this study also found that for 8-year-olds, the visuo-spatial sketchpad was a stronger predictor of mathematics performance than the central executive. Similarly, Gathercole and Pickering (Gathercole & Pickering, 2000) found that 6- and 7-year-old children's performance on national curriculum mathematics assessments correlate with performance on measures of visuo-spatial WM.

Taken together, these studies suggest that WM plays an important role in both mathematical performance and skill development in 7- to 11-year-old children. Current data also hint at the changing role of different WM components in relation to performance and skill development. However, the research to date has been contradictory; some studies implicate the phonological loop, others the visuo-spatial sketchpad, and still others the central executive (Henry and MacLean, 2003, Holmes and Adams, 2006) There are several reasons for such inconsistencies. The first is related to the large age-range across studies (Adams et al., 1998, Andersson, 2007, Durand et al., 2005, Holmes and Adams, 2006, Swanson, 2006), resulting in high variability in the level of the participants' mathematical competence and in the curricular content of the mathematical tasks. To address these issues, we focus on two groups of children in the 2nd and 3rd grades who are at an important stage in formal mathematical skill development.

A second reason for the inconsistencies in findings is the large variability in the types of tasks used to assess mathematical performance. For example, some studies have focused either on individual arithmetic operations, such as addition (Adams & Hitch, 1997), subtraction (Barrouillet, Mignon & Thevenot, 2008) or other more complex arithmetic problems (Henry & MacLean, 2003). The use of a single measure of mathematical ability is useful for studying particular cognitive questions, but is less useful for revealing more general links between WM and developmentally relevant mathematical abilities. To obtain a more complete and ecologically valid profile of mathematics competence, we administered two standardized mathematics achievement measures — the Numerical Operations and Mathematical Reasoning subtests of the Wechsler Individual Achievement Test (WIAT-II; (Wechsler, 2001)). A key distinction between the two measures is that Numerical Operations plays a greater emphasis on counting and computation whereas Mathematical Reasoning emphasizes word problems.

A third reason for inconsistencies across studies is the use of non-standardized instruments to assess WM. We use the Working Memory Test Battery for Children (WMTB-C), a comprehensive, standardized assessment of three core WM components (Pickering & Gathercole, 2001). Importantly, large-scale studies have found that the three-component model of WM best fits empirical data on the structure and development of WM in 6- to 16-year-old children (Gathercole, Pickering, Ambridge & Wearing, 2004). Additionally, WM capacity can be expressed both in terms of raw and standardized scores on each of the three WM components. Raw scores reflect age-related differences, whereas age-normed scores are useful in assessing performance differences after controlling for normative development. Accordingly, we used both raw and age-normed scores from the standardized WMTB-C measure to examine how each WM component influences mathematical abilities assessed by the Numerical Operations and Mathematics Reasoning. Based on previous research on 2nd and 3rd graders' strategy use (Geary et al., 2004, Wu et al., 2008) we hypothesized that 2nd graders would rely more on the central executive compared to the 3rd graders because of the greater use of counting and other algorithmic strategies, whereas 3rd graders would rely more on automated retrieval processes that do not require the central executive to the same extent as 2nd graders.

Section snippets

Participants

Participants were recruited from a wide range of schools in the San Francisco Bay Area using mailings to schools, newspaper advertisements and postings at libraries. Participants include 48 2nd graders (17 girls, 31 boys) between the ages of 7 to 8.4 (M = 7.59 years; SD = 0.52), and 50 3rd graders, 24 girls, 26 boys) between the ages of 7.8 and 9.3 (M = 8.52 years; SD = 0.40). Participants were administered a demographic questionnaire and IQ was assessed using the Wechsler Abbreviated Scales of

IQ, WIAT-II and WMTB-C in 2nd and 3rd graders

Although there was some overlap in the ages of the 2nd and 3rd graders, the two groups differed significantly in age (p < 0.0001). There were no differences between the verbal IQ, performance IQ, and full-scale IQ scores of 2nd and 3rd graders (FSIQ M = 108, SD = 11.39, respectively), as shown in Table 1, suggesting comparable samples. As might be expected, Mathematical Reasoning and Numerical Operations raw scores were significantly higher in 3rd graders than 2nd graders, but there were no

Discussion

The purpose of our study was to clarify how the three WM systems differentially contribute to 2nd and 3rd grade children's performance on standardized measures that assess basic number, counting, and arithmetic competencies (Numerical Operations) and more complex problem solving competencies (Mathematical Reasoning). We hypothesized that the central executive would more strongly predict 2nd graders' than 3rd graders' performance on both mathematics measures. We found that the central executive

Acknowledgements

We thank Kevin Holmes and Katherine Keller for assistance with data acquisition. This research was supported by grants from the National Institutes of Health (NICHD-HD047520 and NICHD-HD059205) and the National Science Foundation (BCS/DRL-075034) to VM. DG acknowledges support from grants from NICHD-HD38283 and NICHD-HD045914, co-funded by NIH and the Office of Special Education and Rehabilitation Services.

References (89)

  • D.C. Geary et al.

    Numerical and arithmetical cognition: A longitudinal study of process and concept deficits in children with learning disability

    Journal of Experimental Child Psychology

    (2000)
  • D. Geary et al.

    Strategy choices in simple and complex addition: Contributions of working memory and counting knowledge for children with mathematical disability

    Journal of Experimental Child Psychology

    (2004)
  • D.C. Geary et al.

    Numerical and arithmetical cognition: Patterns of functions and deficits in children at risk for a mathematical disability

    Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. Special Issue: The Development of Mathematical Cognition: Arithmetic

    (1999)
  • A. Ischebeck et al.

    Imaging early practice effects in arithmetic

    NeuroImage

    (2007)
  • A. Ischebeck et al.

    How specifically do we learn? Imaging the learning of multiplication and subtraction

    NeuroImage

    (2006)
  • N.C. Jordan et al.

    Arithmetic fact mastery in young children: A longitudinal investigation

    Journal of Experimental Child Psychology

    (2003)
  • K. Landerl et al.

    Developmental dyscalculia and basic numerical capacities: A study of 8–9-year-old students

    Cognition

    (2004)
  • S.C. Levine et al.

    Development of calculation abilities in young children

    Journal of Experimental Child Psychology

    (1992)
  • J.F. McLean et al.

    Working memory impairments in children with specific arithmetic learning difficulties

    Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. Special Issue: The Development of Mathematical Cognition: Arithmetic

    (1999)
  • V. Menon et al.

    Dissociating prefrontal and parietal cortex activation during arithmetic processing

    NeuroImage

    (2000)
  • I.T. Miura et al.

    Language supports for children's understanding of numerical fractions: Cross-national comparisons

    Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. Special Issue: The Development of Mathematical Cognition: Numerical Processes and Concepts

    (1999)
  • M.C. Passolunghi et al.

    A comparison of updating processes in children good or poor in arithmetic word problem-solving

    Learning and Individual Differences

    (2005)
  • M.C. Passolunghi et al.

    Short-term memory, working memory, and inhibitory control in children with difficulties in arithmetic problem solving

    Journal of Experimental Child Psychology

    (2001)
  • M.C. Passolunghi et al.

    Working memory and access to numerical information in children with disability in mathematics

    Journal of Experimental Child Psychology

    (2004)
  • C. Rasmussen et al.

    Representation and working memory in early arithmetic

    Journal of Experimental Child Psychology

    (2005)
  • H.L. Swanson

    Working memory in learning disability subgroups

    Journal of Experimental Child Psychology

    (1993)
  • H.L. Swanson et al.

    The influence of working memory and classification ability on children's word problem solution

    Journal of Experimental Child Psychology

    (1993)
  • H.L. Swanson et al.

    Mathematical problem solving and working memory in children with learning disabilities: Both executive and phonological processes are important

    Journal of Experimental Child Psychology

    (2001)
  • T.M. Achenbach

    Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/4-18 and 1991 Profile

    (1991)
  • Adams, J. W., Hitch, G. J., & Donlan, C. (1998). Children's mental arithmetic and working memory. In Anonymous (Ed.),...
  • U. Andersson

    The contribution of working memory to children's mathematical word problem solving

    Applied Cognitive Psychology

    (2007)
  • D. Ansari

    Effects of development and enculturation on number representation in the brain

    Nature Reviews. Neuroscience

    (2008)
  • Baddeley, A. (1986). Working memory. (Vol. 11). New York, NY: Clarendon Press/Oxford University...
  • A.D. Baddeley

    Exploring the central executive

    Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology

    (1996)
  • A. Baddeley et al.

    Random generation and the executive control of working memory

    Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. A

    (1998)
  • A.D. Baddeley et al.

    Working memory: The multi-component model

  • R. Bull et al.

    Short-term memory, working memory, and executive functioning in preschoolers: Longitudinal predictors of mathematical achievement at age 7 years

    Developmental Neuropsychology

    (2008)
  • R. Bull et al.

    Exploring the roles of the visual–spatial sketch pad and central executive in children's arithmetical skills: Views from cognition and developmental neuropsychology

    Developmental Neuropsychology

    (1999)
  • V.J. Dark et al.

    Enhanced problem translation and short-term memory: Components of mathematical talent

    Journal of Educational Psychology

    (1990)
  • S. De Rammelaere et al.

    The contribution of working memory resources in the verification of simple mental arithmetic sums

    Psychological Research

    (1999)
  • S. De Rammelaere et al.

    Verifying simple arithmetic sums and products: Are the phonolgical loop and the central executive involved?

    Memory & Cognition

    (2001)
  • P.A. Frensch et al.

    Effects of practice on component processes in complex mental addition

    Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition

    (1993)
  • L.S. Fuchs et al.

    The prevention, identification, and cognitive determinants of math difficulty

    Journal of Educational Psychology

    (2005)
  • A.J. Furst et al.

    Separate roles for executive and phonological components of working memory in mental arithmetic

    Memory & Cognition

    (2000)
  • Cited by (0)

    1

    Both contributed equally to the manuscript as second authors.

    View full text