The influence of self-efficacy and working memory capacity on problem-solving efficiency

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2008.08.001Get rights and content

Abstract

We investigated the influence of self-efficacy beliefs and working memory capacity on mathematical problem-solving performance, response time, and efficiency (i.e., the ratio of problems solved correctly to time). Students completed a letter-recoding task (Experiment 1) or an operation span task (Experiment 2), rated their self-efficacy for solving mental multiplication problems, and then solved similar problems of varying complexity. We tested the motivational efficiency hypothesis, which predicted that motivational beliefs, such as self-efficacy, increase problem-solving efficiency through focused effort and strategy use. Experiments 1 and 2 reported a significant effect for self-efficacy on problem-solving performance and efficiency, but limited effects for time. A self-efficacy by working memory interaction occurred in Experiment 1, suggesting self-efficacy is beneficial as demands on working memory increase. These findings suggested that self-efficacy increased problem-solving efficiency through strategic performance rather than faster solution times, and were consistent with the motivational efficiency hypothesis.

Introduction

Solving problems accurately and quickly continues to be the hallmark of efficient learners. Problem solving has been studied for decades and a great deal is known about general problem solving (Anderson, 1993, Dewey, 1910, Mayer, 1998), as well as task-specific problem solving in a variety of domains such as mathematics (Ashcraft, 1992, Campbell and Hackett, 1986, Kaye et al., 1989, Royer et al., 1999). Both task- and domain knowledge are essential components of problem-solving accuracy, however, a variety of other variables are related to problem-solving accuracy above and beyond domain knowledge, including self-efficacy beliefs (Lopez et al., 1997, Pajares and Kranzler, 1995, Pajares and Miller, 1994), working memory capacity (Adams and Hitch, 1997, DeStefano and LeFevre, 2004, Klein and Bisanz, 2000, Seitz and Schumann-Hengsteler, 2000, Swanson, 2004, Swanson and Beebe-Frankenberger, 2004), and problem complexity (Campbell and Xue, 2001, Hitch, 1978, Logie et al., 1994, Mabbott and Bisanz, 2003).

Surprisingly, few studies have examined the role of motivational variables and variables such as working memory capacity, or problem complexity on problem-solving efficiency, which we define as the ratio of the number of problems solved correctly to the amount of time needed to solve them (Mory, 1992). Of special importance to the current research, there are no studies we know of that specifically have examined the influence of self-efficacy beliefs on problem-solving efficiency. The need for efficiency is important as some problem solving situations, such as post-secondary instruction, impose rigid time constraints.

The main goal of the present study was to examine whether self-efficacy beliefs enhance problem-solving efficiency of mental multiplication, while controlling for the variables of working memory capacity and problem complexity. This question is important for both theoretical and practical reasons. From a theoretical perspective, it is unknown whether self-efficacy increases problem-solving efficiency, even though self-efficacy is related to math problem-solving accuracy (Lopez et al., 1997, Pajares and Kranzler, 1995, Pajares and Miller, 1994). We tested the motivational efficiency hypothesis (Hoffman & Spatariu, 2008), which predicted that motivational beliefs, such as self-efficacy, increase problem-solving efficiency. It is possible that self-efficacy increases, decreases, or has no effect on response time. Although prior research has shown the positive influence of self-efficacy upon problem-solving accuracy, empirical support for problem-solving efficiency outcomes is lacking.

From a practical perspective, the possible positive effect of self-efficacy on problem-solving efficiency has important implications for understanding the optimal use of limited cognitive resources (Mayer and Moreno, 2003, Paas and van Merriënboer, 1993, Van Gerven et al., 2002). Given that many classrooms have constraints on the problem-solving process, and especially limited time (Marks, 2000), outcomes that seek to understand the relationship between motivational variables and efficiency warrant investigation.

Self-efficacy, defined as the belief in one's ability to organize and execute courses of action to meet desired outcomes (Bandura, 1986), is a powerful intervening variable. The belief that one can perform a task successfully, in many cases, is a better predictor of eventual performance than previous attainments (Bandura, 1986, Pajares, 2002, Pajares, 2003). Specifically, when controlling for performance accuracy, self-efficacy beliefs have been shown to operate independently of underlying skills, and mediate individual difference variables such as background knowledge (Pajares, 2003, Pajares and Miller, 1994), metacognitive awareness (McCombs and Marzano, 1990, Schunk and Ertmer, 2000), and overall ability (Bandura, 1986, Campbell and Hackett, 1986, Pajares and Kranzler, 1995, Pajares and Miller, 1994).

Self-efficacy beliefs play a powerful role concerning choice, persistence, effort, strategy use, and interest in mathematical problem solving (Lopez et al., 1997, Pajares and Kranzler, 1995, Pajares and Miller, 1994). Self-efficacy for math performance has been linked to selection of a college major (Hackett, 1985), and holds implications for mathematics career choice (Hackett & Betz, 1989). In addition, self-efficacy is positively related to mathematics achievement and problem solving over and above mathematics ability (Pajares and Kranzler, 1995, Pajares and Miller, 1994). Path analysis studies emphasize the predictive ability of self-efficacy by demonstrating self-efficacy either accounts for unique variance beyond factors such as gender, prior experience, perceived usefulness of math and self-concept (Pajares & Miller, 1994), or when controlling for background knowledge (Pajares & Kranzler, 1995). Although ability beliefs are a critical component of self-efficacy assessments, domain interest may be more of a reflection upon outcomes than capability (Lopez et al., 1997). Several meta-analyses have shown that effect sizes attributed to the mediating influence of self-efficacy on performance has ranged from .08 to .38 (Multon et al., 1991, Stajkovic and Luthans, 1998, Valentine et al., 2004).

Self-efficacy persists over time (Pajares & Graham, 1999), although judgments may change as the perception of task difficulty fluctuates. For example, Stajkovic and Luthans (1998) reported the role of self-efficacy diminished as task complexity increased. Campbell and Hackett (1986) found that self-efficacy assessments for easy tasks are higher due to the expectations of greater task success. However, it is not clear from these studies whether task difficulty and self-efficacy are related to problem-solving efficiency.

Self-efficacy is related to the tactics students will use when solving problems. Bandura (1986) described the connection between a learner's ability to control the learning environment and self-regulation. The sense of control, in turn, enhances the belief about their capabilities and potential to control their destiny (Pajares, 2002). The learner who believes s/he is capable of goal attainment uses more productive metacognitive strategies (Butler & Winne, 1995), works harder, expends more effort, and persists longer (Bandura, 1986, Bouffard-Bouchard, 2001; Lodewyk and Winne, 2005, Schunk and Zimmerman, 2006).

The self-efficacy research mentioned implies two main conclusions. First, self-efficacy is a powerful individual difference variable that is strongly related to academic achievement (Pajares, 1996, Zimmerman et al., 1992). Strong self-efficacy beliefs can minimize other individual difference factors such as anxiety, physiological predisposition, and interest (Lent, Lopez, Brown, & Gore, 1996).

Second, self-efficacy influences performance beyond existing skills and ability. Even when controlling for general intelligence (Pajares & Kranzler, 1995) or prior math experience (Pajares & Miller, 1994), judgments of self-efficacy predicted achievement outcomes. The beliefs individuals possess concerning anticipated success also determine what challenges individuals attempt (Pajares & Kranzler, 1995). In sum, these studies demonstrate a significant influence of self-efficacy on performance outcomes.

Mental arithmetic requires the problem solver to encode the presented information, perform a mental calculation, and provide a response (DeStefano and LeFevre, 2004, Logie et al., 1994). Solving of mental arithmetic involves cognitive processes beyond mere fact retrieval (Seitz & Schumann-Hengsteler, 2000), and is assumed to include both the storage and processing of information (Hitch, 1978, Mabbott and Bisanz, 2003, Swanson and Beebe-Frankenberger, 2004).

Previous research indicates that individuals with higher levels of working memory capacity (WMC) perform better on learning tasks because they have more cognitive resources (Daneman and Carpenter, 1980, Mayer, 2001, Mousavi et al., 1995). WMC also is positively correlated with general fluid intelligence, (g), (Engle, Kane, & Tuholski, 1999) and speed of processing (Bjorklund, 2005). It is likely that WMC affects cognitive efficiency due to the processing and storage requirements necessary to solve mental problems.

The effect of WMC on math problem solving has been documented in many studies (Adams and Hitch, 1997, DeStefano and LeFevre, 2004, Klein and Bisanz, 2000, Seitz and Schumann-Hengsteler, 2000, Swanson, 2004, Swanson and Beebe-Frankenberger, 2004). The current research used an operations span task to measure WMC and a recoding task to measure short-term memory. Span tasks require participants to solve problems while concurrently remembering either the cumulative sums of a series of problems or a list of words or numbers that follow a sequence of problems. These tasks presumably measure both a basic retrieval mechanism as well as central executive processing (Engle et al., 1999, Swanson, 2004, Swanson and Beebe-Frankenberger, 2004). In contrast, a letter-recoding task (Benton, Kraft, Glover, & Plake, 1984) provides a measure of basic storage, such as temporary storage of verbal information in Baddeley's (1998) articulatory loop.

The processing efficiency of working memory is contingent upon both problem complexity (number of digits) and task demand (carry and load demands) (DeStefano & LeFevre, 2004). Kaye et al. (1989) used a dual-task paradigm requiring problem solving and concurrent detection of auditory probes. Dual task conditions are designed to divert memory resources from the primary task of remembering problem solutions. Kaye et al. concluded when participants were required to maintain sums in memory, or attend to dual tasks, response time increased. In a related study, Logie et al. (1994) used adding-span techniques, which involve addition of individual problems while concurrently maintaining a cumulative running total, to study mental arithmetic. Volunteer participants were required to solve either “single carry” or “multiple carry” (p. 399) mental arithmetic problems in both single and dual-task conditions. These studies demonstrated that problem-solving ability was related to the activation of working memory resources, including the central executive, which generally is regarded as the active processing component of working memory (DeStefano & LeFevre, 2004). They also supported the conclusion that as task complexity increases, processing become less efficient by imposing greater demands upon working memory that impede performance (Pollock et al., 2002, Sweller and Chandler, 1994).

A critical variable influencing problem-solving performance is problem complexity. Solving basic multiplication problems, such as 3 × 4, often involves use of an automated calculation algorithm (Logie et al., 1994) that requires few working memory resources. Solving problems of multiple digits involves greater complexity (Hitch, 1978, Logie et al., 1994, Mabbott and Bisanz, 2003), takes longer (Hitch, 1978, Royer et al., 1999, Siegler, 1988), and requires time consuming mental computations (Campbell & Xue, 2001) resulting in far greater demand on limited cognitive resources.

The research on WMC leads to two main conclusions. First, math problem-solving ability is mediated by WMC (DeStefano and LeFevre, 2004, Logie et al., 1994, Passolunghi and Siegel, 2001, Swanson, 2004, Swanson and Beebe-Frankenberger, 2004). WMC influences how material is encoded, strategies used, problem-solving time, and overall performance accuracy.

Second, problem complexity and problem length determine the efficiency of problem-solving performance (Adams and Hitch, 1997, Campbell and Xue, 2001, Siegler, 1988). Problems requiring multiple stages of calculation take longer to solve, result in reduced accuracy, and presumably use more working memory resources. Thus, more complex problems may be solved less efficiently (Ashcraft, 1992, Kaye et al., 1989, Logie et al., 1994). Determining the processing limitations imposed by time, ability, and instructional constraints constitutes an important step in defining overall problem-solving efficiency.

The present study investigated whether self-efficacy beliefs are related to problem-solving performance, problem-solving time, and problem-solving efficiency under unrestricted time conditions. We controlled for the effect of working memory capacity and problem complexity to better examine the unique variation shared by self-efficacy, performance accuracy, and efficiency. Before solving a series of multiplication problems, participants assessed their self-efficacy for mental multiplication accuracy, and completed either an alphabetic recoding task in Experiment 1 or an operation span task in Experiment 2. Problem complexity was manipulated using four difficulty levels of math problems in Experiment 1 and two levels in Experiment 2.

We proposed the motivational efficiency hypothesis (Hoffman & Spatariu, 2008), which states that positive motivational beliefs such as self-efficacy (Bandura, 1986, Bandura, 1997, Butler and Winne, 1995, Pintrich, 2000, Pintrich and De Groot, 1990), personal goal orientations (Ames and Archer, 1988, Stone, 2000), intrinsic motivation (Zimmerman, 1989), engagement, or attributions to metacognitive strategy use (Fredericks et al., 2004, Linnenbrink and Pintrich, 2003) are related to more efficient problem solving. Our primary goal was to investigate whether self-efficacy is related to efficiency in addition to performance. Previous research has failed to investigate this important question. We believe that self-efficacy is related to better problem-solving efficiency for three inter-related reasons. One reason is that higher self-efficacy individuals use the most appropriate problem-solving strategy (Siegler, 1988, Walczyk, 1994). A second reason is individuals with greater self-efficacy also engage in attentional focusing, in which they assess the complexity of the problem and monitor their solution in the most efficient manner (Cowan, 2005, Mayer and Moreno, 2003). A third reason is that higher self-efficacy individuals experience fewer distractions due to anxiety and handicapping (Ashcraft and Kirk, 2001, Eysenck and Calvo, 1992, Hopko et al., 2005).

We made several predictions regarding the relationship between self-efficacy and WMC. First, we expected individuals with higher self-efficacy and greater WMC to be more efficient, solving problems both more accurately and quickly. In addition, individuals should be more efficient solving easier problems (e.g., one digit multiplied by two digits) than more difficult ones (e.g., two digits multiplied by two digits). A second prediction was that self-efficacy would interact with WMC in a compensatory manner. We expected self-efficacy to influence performance and efficiency more when WMC was low.

Section snippets

Participants

Study participants were students enrolled in an introductory educational psychology course from a large southwestern university in the United States (N = 58, 16 = males, 42 = females), who volunteered as partial fulfillment of a class requirement. The study employed a regression design to assess the relationships among self-efficacy and WMC on three outcome measures. Each participant solved 32 problems of mental multiplication at four different levels of problem complexity. No subject attrition was

Results

Preliminary t-tests examined if results related to problem-solving performance and problem-solving response time were influenced by gender. Results indicated gender did not influence problem-solving performance or problem-solving time, t(56) =− 1.767, p = .083 (Mfemale = 17.39, SDfemale = 6.06 and Mmale = 14.06, SDmale = 7.21) and t(56) =  1.269, p = .403, (Mfemale = 1412.15, SDfemale = 281.58 and Mmale = 1346.14, SDmale = 373.47) respectively.

Summary of results—Experiment 1

Our findings revealed a two-way interaction between self-efficacy and WMC on performance. This result suggested the combination of high WMC and high self-efficacy was related to improved problem solving. Also as problem complexity increased, self-efficacy became a better predictor of problem-solving accuracy. Self-efficacy accounted for a significant proportion of variance in performance accuracy and efficiency at high levels of problem complexity over and above the effect of other variables.

Experiment 2

Experiment 2 was conducted for two reasons. First, Experiment 2 utilized an operation span working memory task, which is thought to provide a more comprehensive method of measuring working memory capacity (Engle et al., 1999, Engle et al., 1999; Swanson, 2004, Swanson and Beebe-Frankenberger, 2004). It is possible that a measure of memory span may be sensitive to changes in efficiency that the measure of letter recoding did not detect. Second, we used two levels of problem difficulty with more

Participants

Participants consisted of 81 (21 males and 60 females) college undergraduates from the pool described in Experiment 1. Participants volunteered as partial fulfillment of a class requirement. No subject attrition was encountered.

Materials and procedures

The methodology for Experiment 2 was the same as Experiment 1 with two exceptions, the working memory measure and levels of complexity. First, the working memory measure employed was an operation span working memory task, employing the methodology and scoring procedure

Results

Problem-solving performance, time and efficiency were calculated and analyzed as in Experiment 1. Results were screened for violation of assumptions and outliers, resulting in two cases being removed from the statistical analysis due to excessive problem-solving time. A preliminary comparison of males and females was performed using problem-solving performance and problem-solving response time. Both analyses indicated that gender did not affect results on the dependent measures, t(77) =  1.986, p =

Summary of results—Experiment 2

As in Experiment 1, individuals with higher levels of self-efficacy solved more problems correctly and were more efficient in the problem-solving process. As problem complexity increased, the influence of self-efficacy as a predictor of performance accuracy and efficiency increased.

WMC was unrelated to any of the three outcome measures. These findings indicate that the working memory operation span task, which measures complex processing capacity, did not predict performance on any of the

General discussion

This study investigated the effect of self-efficacy and WMC at multiple levels of problem complexity on problem-solving performance, response time, and problem-solving efficiency. We tested the motivational efficiency hypothesis, which states that motivational beliefs such as self-efficacy, are related to problem-solving efficiency. Although previous research has established a clear relationship between self-efficacy and performance, few studies have investigated the relationship between

References (80)

  • PassolunghiM.C. et al.

    Short-term memory, working memory, and inhibitory control in children with difficulties in arithmetic problem-solving

    Journal of Experimental Child Psychology

    (2001)
  • PollockE. et al.

    Assimilating complex information

    Learning and Instruction

    (2002)
  • RoyerJ.M. et al.

    Math-fact retrieval as the cognitive mechanism underlying gender differences in math test performance

    Contemporary Educational Psychology

    (1999)
  • SchunkD. et al.

    Self-regulation and academic learning: Self-efficacy enhancing interventions

  • Van GervenP.W.M. et al.

    Cognitive load theory and aging: Effects of worked examples on training efficiency

    Learning and Instruction

    (2002)
  • AikenL.S. et al.

    Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions

    (1991)
  • AmesC. et al.

    Achievement goals in the classroom: Students' learning strategies and motivation processes

    Journal of Educational Psychology

    (1988)
  • AndersonJ.R.

    Problem-solving and learning

    American Psychologist

    (1993)
  • AshcraftM.H. et al.

    The relationships among working memory, math anxiety and performance

    Journal of Experimental Psychology. General

    (2001)
  • BaddeleyA.D.

    Human memory: Theory and practice

    (1998)
  • BanduraA.

    Social foundations of thought and action: A social cognitive theory

    (1986)
  • BanduraA.

    Self-efficacy: The exercise of control

    (1997)
  • BentonS.L. et al.

    Cognitive capacity differences among writers

    Journal of Educational Psychology

    (1984)
  • BjorklundD.

    Children's thinking: Cognitive development and individual differences

    (2005)
  • Bouffard-BouchardT.

    Influence of self-efficacy on performance in a cognitive task

    The Journal of Social Psychology

    (2001)
  • ButlerD.L. et al.

    Feedback and self-regulated learning: A theoretical synthesis

    Review of Educational Research

    (1995)
  • CampbellJ.I.D. et al.

    Cognitive arithmetic across cultures

    Journal of Experimental Psychology. General

    (2001)
  • ChenP.

    Exploring the accuracy and predictability of the self-efficacy beliefs of seventh-grade mathematics students

    Learning and Individual Differences

    (2002)
  • ConwayA.R.A. et al.

    Working memory span tasks: A methodological review and user's guide

    Psychonomic Bulletin & Review

    (2005)
  • CowanN.

    Working memory capacity

    (2005)
  • DeStefanoD. et al.

    The role of working memory in mental arithmetic

    European Journal of Cognitive Psychology

    (2004)
  • DeweyJ.

    How we think

    (1910)
  • EngleR.W. et al.

    Individual differences in working memory capacity and what they tell us about controlled attention, general fluid intelligence and function of the prefrontal cortex

  • EngleR.W. et al.

    Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: A latent-variable approach

    Journal of Experimental Psychology. General

    (1999)
  • EysenckM.W. et al.

    Anxiety and performance: The processing efficiency theory

    Cognition and Emotion

    (1992)
  • FredericksJ.A. et al.

    School engagement: Potential of the concept, state of the evidence

    Review of Educational Research

    (2004)
  • HackettG.

    Role of mathematics self-efficacy in the choice of math-related majors of college women and men: A path analysis

    Journal of Counseling Psychology

    (1985)
  • HackettG. et al.

    An exploration of the mathematics self-efficacy/mathematics performance correspondence

    Journal for Research in Mathematics Education

    (1989)
  • HambrickD.Z. et al.

    The role of working memory in higher-level cognition: Domain specific versus domain-general perspectives

  • HechtS.A.

    Counting on working memory in simple arithmetic when counting is used for problem-solving

    Memory & Cognition

    (2002)
  • Cited by (88)

    • Self-motivated and directed learning across the lifespan

      2023, Acta Psychologica
      Citation Excerpt :

      One study found an interaction between working memory and self-efficacy for efficient mathematical problem-solving performance. The authors concluded that self-efficacy boosted the learner's problem-solving performance as demands on working memory increased (Hoffman & Schraw, 2009). Though this study occurred under controlled, other-directed conditions, the results are helpful in thinking about how self-efficacy may drive working memory under self-motivated and directed conditions.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text