Do performance avoidance goals moderate the effect of different types of stereotype threat on women’s math performance?

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2016.05.009Get rights and content

Highlights

  • This paper is a close replication of Chalabaev, Major, Sarrazin, and Cury (2012).

  • We tested for an interaction between stereotype threat and achievement goals.

  • We found no significant main or interaction effects for threat or goals.

  • No significant effects emerged despite multiple controls.

  • We suggest that the effect of stereotype threat may be weaker than commonly thought.

Abstract

Stereotype threat is considered to be a robust effect that explains persistent gender gaps in math performance and scientific career trajectories. Some evidence suggests stereotype threat effects are buffered by adoption of performance avoidance goals (Chalabaev, Major, Sarrazin, & Cury, 2012). With 590 American female participants, we closely replicated Chalabaev et al. (2012). Results showed no significant main or interaction effects for stereotype threat or performance avoidance goals, despite multiple controls. We conclude that effects of stereotype threat might be smaller than typically reported and find limited evidence for moderation by avoidance achievement goals. Accordingly, stereotype threat might not be a major part of the explanation for the gender gap in math performance, consistent with recent meta-analyses (Flore & Wicherts, 2015).

Introduction

Despite research suggesting girls receive higher marks than boys across all subjects, including math and science courses, girls in the United States score lower than boys on the math section of the SAT (College Board, 2013; d = −0.27), and make up only 25% of the STEM (science, technology, engineering, and mathematics) workforce when women make up 47% of the overall workforce (National Math & Science Initiative, 2013). Given the importance of standardized tests for college admissions, the need for research to accurately identify the forces behind women’s poorer performance on tests of math ability is imperative for optimal interventions and policy changes. The present research aimed to replicate the effect of stereotype threat on math performance and to determine how different types of stereotype threat impact performance. Few direct replications of stereotype threat have been published, and we aim to test this effect in a large online sample of American women. Further, we tested how threat interacts with performance avoidance goals, which are defined as aiming at avoiding poor performance relative to one’s peers (Elliot & McGregor, 2001).

One of the most prominent psychological theories used to explain the math-achievement gap is stereotype threat, which occurs when individuals are fearful of confirming negative stereotypes associated with their group. Steele and Aaronson (1995) posit that “in situations where the stereotype is applicable, one is at risk of confirming it as a self-characterization, both to one’s self and to others who know the stereotype” (p. 808). This theory was applied to the women in mathematics stereotype by Spencer, Steele, and Quinn (1999)—a landmark paper that illustrated the negative effect of framing a test as diagnostic of math ability or gender differences on female math performance. Many studies have used cues stemming from Spencer et al., ranging from test framings to threatening test administrators (Shapiro & Neuberg, 2007). Recent research has attempted to make sense of these different types of stereotype threat and how they distinctly impact performance.

Shapiro and Neuberg’s (2007) multi-threat framework classifies threats based on the threat’s source and target. The three sources of stereotype threat are self, outgroup (e.g., men), and ingroup (e.g., other women in mathematics). The two targets of threat are the self and the group (e.g., an individual woman is the self, the female gender as a whole is the group). The authors theorize that these threats have different effects on individuals; for example, group-as-target threats are believed to lead to more negative comparisons between the self and ingroup and outgroup members than self-as-target threats do. The current study focuses on two “self-as-source” threats: group-concept threat and self-concept threat. Group-concept threat targets the stereotyped group to which one belongs and differentiates the stereotyped group from the majority (e.g., men in mathematics). Group-concept threats thus confirm, to a woman in mathematics, that her gender as a whole is bad at math. Self-concept threat is the fear that one’s performance will confirm that oneself is bad at math. Unlike group-concept threat, self-concept threat targets one’s inherent attributes or skills, and self-concept threat does not require identification with the group (Shapiro & Neuberg, 2007). Using two threat manipulations, we aimed to experimentally test if self-concept threat or group-concept threat differentially affect math test performance and if these threats interact with variables believed to moderate the effect of stereotype threat (e.g., gender identity and math identity).

In the last decade, stereotype threat research has shifted in focus from investigating the existence of stereotypes to examining the mechanisms that influence test performance for women (Shapiro & Williams, 2012). Performance avoidance goals have been identified as a potential consequence of stereotype threat for women in mathematics because individuals endorse those goals to avoid performing poorly in comparison to peers (Elliot & McGregor, 2001). For example, if a student aims to prevent scoring low on the SAT in comparison to her friends and other students she knows, she adopts a performance avoidance goal orientation. Performance avoidance goals and stereotype threat are associated with anxiety, fear of failure, and negative evaluations of self-competence, and thus they typically impair performance (Smith, Sansone, & White, 2007).

However, individuals who adopt performance-avoidance goals are in a state similar to those completing tasks that are inherently threatening, and theory suggests that matching goal orientation to task characteristics boosts performance (Cesario, Higgins, & Scholer, 2008). Stereotype threat (particularly group-concept threat) emphasizes social comparisons, a feature that is an integral component of performance-avoidance goals. Grimm, Markman, Maddox, and Baldwin (2009) posit that an avoidance goal orientation matches a stereotype threat situation better than an approach goal orientation does; this fit may then lead stereotyped individuals to use cognitive strategies that help them avoid negative outcomes and perform as well as non-threatened individuals.

This matching effect has been tested and found in studies on stereotype threat for women in math. Chalabaev, Major, Sarrazin, and Cury (2012) operationalized stereotype threat through a “math ability” cue (self-concept threat) in one study and a “gender differences” cue (group-concept threat) in a second study, in accordance with Spencer et al.’s (1999) manipulations. Both of Chalabaev et al.’s studies demonstrated that inducing a performance-avoidance goal produces better performance under stereotype threat on a math test when compared to no goal, an effect subsequently detected by others (Deemer, Smith, Carroll, & Carpenter, 2014).

Given the theoretical links between stereotype threat and performance-avoidance goals and the evidence supporting their interaction, we sought (1) to test this interaction in a large-scale, pre-registered replication, using Chalabaev et al.’s (2012) basic design, and (2) to extend their design to directly compare two types of threat from Shapiro and Neuberg’s (2007) framework. Additionally, we tested for differences in magnitude of effect between self-concept and group-concept threat with various moderators (detailed below). We predicted that we would find a stereotype threat by performance avoidance goal interaction, such that women under threat would perform better on a math test with a performance avoidance goal compared to no goal. We also predicted that the effect of group-concept threat would be stronger when compared to self-concept threat. We tested these hypotheses with an all-female sample, because the performance of males is not inhibited by stereotype threat in the domain of mathematics (Walton & Cohen, 2003).

Replications of stereotype threat studies are relatively uncommon, and we aimed to test this effect in a larger sample than the vast majority of research in this area (Stoet & Geary, 2012). Policymakers and social psychologists point to stereotype threat as a major force behind the gender gap in STEM, although there are few tests of stereotype threat in this domain on samples of non-undergraduates (Flore & Wicherts, 2015). By conducting highly-powered test of stereotype threat in a diverse sample, this study will provide insight into the generalizability of stereotype threat, and it will test the extent to which threat can impact performance in an online setting.

Section snippets

Pilot study & design

We pre-registered our design and analysis plan (see https://osf.io/kms6g). In order to measure math performance under differing conditions of stereotype threat, we used 10 released questions from the GRE quantitative reasoning section, also used in Chalabaev et al. (2012). We ran a pilot study testing math ability with three tests of different degrees of difficulty (i.e., sixth grade, tenth grade, and the GRE) to ensure that these questions were appropriately difficult for the intended audience

Results

All statistical analyses were completed in R Statistical Software (Version 3.1.1), and the script for the analyses is available at https://osf.io/xfs9d/, and the data file itself is at https://osf.io/pabns/. 95% confidence intervals for effect sizes were calculated using the compute.es package in R (Del Re, 2014). Participants were randomly assigned to one out of six possible conditions: Math Ability/Performance Avoidance goal (N = 103), Math Ability/No goal (N = 92), Gender Differences/Performance

Discussion

The current study aimed to assess the impact of different types of stereotype threat in a large sample of adult women and to determine if inducing performance-avoidance goals improves performance under threat. Our results indicate a failure to replicate Chalabaev et al. (2012), with no evidence suggesting the presence of significant stereotype threat main effects, nor any moderation by performance avoidance goals, in spite of the fact that the current replication study had a much larger sample

Author contributions

K. M. F. developed the study concept and design and collected the data, under the supervision of K. S. C. K. M. F. conducted data analysis and interpretation; K. S. C independently checked all analyses. K. M. F. drafted the manuscript, and the authors revised the draft together. Both authors approved the final version of the manuscript for submission.

Conflict of interest

The authors declared no potential conflicts of interest and received no financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

References (37)

  • A. Chalabaev et al.

    When avoiding failure improves performance: Stereotype threat and the impact of performance goals

    Motivation and Emotion

    (2012)
  • S. Champley

    pwr: Basic functions for power analysis. R package version 1.1.1

  • College Board

    2013 College-bound seniors total group profile report

  • J.L. Cundiff et al.

    Do gender–science stereotypes predict science identification and science career aspirations among undergraduate science majors?

    Social Psychology of Education

    (2013)
  • E.D. Deemer et al.

    Academic procrastination in STEM: Interactive effects of stereotype threat and achievement goals

    The Career Development Quarterly

    (2014)
  • A.C. Del Re

    compute.es: Compute effect sizes. R package version 0.4

  • A.B. Diekman et al.

    Malleability in communal goals and beliefs influences attraction to stem careers: Evidence for a goal congruity perspective

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (2011)
  • A.J. Elliot et al.

    A 2 × 2 achievement goal framework

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (2001)
  • Cited by (39)

    • The uniformity of stereotype threat: Analyzing the moderating effects of premeasured performance

      2022, Intelligence
      Citation Excerpt :

      To find evidence of performance decrements among test-takers who identify with negatively stereotyped groups, experiments have been conducted that varied the pressure created by negative stereotypes. Despite concerns about potential publication bias (Flore & Wicherts, 2015; Zigerell, 2017), and notwithstanding failures to replicate the effect in well-controlled studies (Agnoli et al., 2021; Finnigan & Corker, 2016; Flore et al., 2018; Gibson et al., 2014; Moon & Roeder, 2014) and in operational tests (Stricker & Ward, 2004), meta-analyses demonstrated that stereotype threat indeed lowers mean test performance of negatively-stereotyped groups in lab settings (e.g., Doyle & Voyer, 2016; Nguyen & Ryan, 2008). The question of whether stereotype threat also affects performance on high-stakes operational tests remains controversial (Sackett & Ryan, 2012; Shewach et al., 2019; Walton & Spencer, 2009).

    • Longitudinal prediction of children's math anxiety from parent-child relationships

      2021, Learning and Individual Differences
      Citation Excerpt :

      Second, fathers often report higher self-concepts of mathematics ability and more intrinsic value of the domain of mathematics (Finnigan & Corker, 2016; Spencer et al., 1999). Research has supported the negative gender stereotype, i.e., women have weaker math abilities, and gender differences in the emotions they experience in mathematics (Finnigan & Corker, 2016; Luong & Knobloch-Westerwick, 2017; Spencer et al., 1999). Additionally, studies have shown that females reported more maladaptive emotions, such as anxiety, shame, and hopelessness, and less enjoyment (Frenzel et al., 2007; Goetz et al., 2013).

    • Under threat but engaged: Stereotype threat leads women to engage with female but not male partners in math

      2019, Contemporary Educational Psychology
      Citation Excerpt :

      As noted above, historically, the predominant outcome in studies on stereotype threat has been performance. Recent attempts to replicate the effect of stereotype threat on math performance when working alone have been mixed; some research has found no effects of stereotype threat on women’s performance (relative to women who did not experience stereotype threat; among adults: Finnigan & Corker, 2016; among 4th through 12th graders: Ganley et al., 2013; among undergraduates: Gibson, Losee, & Vitiello, 2014), and some has found negative effects (i.e., stereotype threat hinders performance; among undergraduates: Schmader, 2002; Schmader & Johns, 2003; Spencer et al., 1999). In addition, a recent meta-analysis that focuses on the effects of stereotype threat for children on math performance found some evidence of an effect (Flore & Wicherts, 2015), which they interpret with caution given evidence of a strong file drawer effect.

    • Psychology as Science and as Propaganda

      2023, Psychology Learning and Teaching
    • Science Communication and the Problematic Impact of Descriptive Norms

      2023, British Journal for the Philosophy of Science
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text