Correlated change of Big Five personality traits across the lifespan: A search for determinants

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2013.08.004Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Correlated change between Big Five dimensions was found across two studies.

  • Correlated change increased after age 70.

  • Correlated change was greater among traits linked to the same developmental process.

  • Cognitive ability was negatively associated with correlated change.

Abstract

Correlated change between different personality traits has recently caught the attention of researchers studying personality development. We conducted two studies to examine age effects (Study 1) and effects of cognitive ability (Study 2) on this phenomenon. Results indicated that correlated change was relatively stable from adolescence through adulthood, and then increased after age 70. Second, correlated change was greater among traits that have been linked to the same developmental processes (e.g., social investment or maturation of specific neurological systems). Third, cognitive ability was negatively associated with correlated change. Collectively, our findings suggest that personality change is partly driven by broad mechanisms affecting multiple traits. Associations with age and cognitive ability provide important leads regarding the possible nature of these mechanisms.

Introduction

Research on personality stability and change is flourishing (Denissen, van Aken, & Roberts, 2011). Knowledge about rank-order and mean-level personality development in adulthood has assumed a more concrete shape (Roberts and DelVecchio, 2000, Roberts et al., 2006). One phenomenon that has recently attracted the field’s attention concerns the question of whether changes in different personality traits are interrelated or independent across individuals, also known as the degree of correlated change. Correlated change concerns the degree to which changes in the level of one trait are related to changes in the level of another trait. For example, positive correlated change between two personality traits, such as agreeableness and conscientiousness, would indicate that the same individuals who show substantive increases in agreeableness also increase in conscientious, whereas individuals showing decreases in agreeableness also decrease in conscientiousness.

Correlated change promises interesting insights into the nature and underlying mechanisms of personality development. According to Soto and John (2012), the degree of correlated change between personality traits can be considered an informative measure indicating whether adult personality development is predominantly influenced by broadly acting mechanisms that simultaneously affect multiple trait domains, or by narrowly acting mechanisms each affecting only one single trait domain.

It should be noted that correlated change is different from trait differentiation, which refers to the extent to which traits are correlated with one another at a certain point in time. Yet, patterns of correlated change are often similar to patterns of trait differentiation (e.g., Allemand, Zimprich, & Martin, 2008). If this is the case, it is likely that a factor that caused the initial correlation simply persists across time. However, it is also possible that previously correlated traits show no correlated change, while previously uncorrelated traits may show correlated change. The first situation could indicate that a causal factor that is shared by two traits is removed or at least does not lead to similar changes in two traits across a particular period, whereas the latter situation could indicate that a new shared causal factor is introduced. Thus, correlated change and trait differentiation are two analytically independent concepts. Whereas the degree of trait differentiation is particularly informative with regard to question about the structure of personality, correlated change is important for gaining greater insight into the dynamics of personality development. The present article was dedicated to study the nature and meaning of correlated change in personality traits.

Only a handful of studies (Allemand et al., 2007, Allemand et al., 2008, Soto and John, 2012) have explicitly examined this phenomenon, yielding inconsistent results. Therefore, our main research question, addressed in two large-scale longitudinal studies, was to first examine whether or not there was consistent evidence for correlated change. The heterogeneous findings of previous studies may also point to the existence of moderator factors. Our two-study design allowed us to examine two of such moderators. In Study 1 we examined how the degree of correlated change varied with age. In Study 2, we examined whether possible age-effects could be explained by differences in cognitive abilities. In both studies, we also examined whether there were specific trait pairs among which evidence for correlated change was stronger.

A number of broadly acting mechanisms affecting multiple trait domains have been proposed as sources of correlated change. There are, for instance, several theoretical reasons to expect associations among changes in agreeableness, conscientiousness, and emotional stability. Digman (1997) proposed that changing to a personality profile consistent with socially approved behavior would be reflected in mean-level increases in the three aforementioned trait-domains. Thus, socialization might be a broad-acting developmental process responsible for correlated change among these traits. In a somewhat related manner, a prominent theoretical principle of personality development, the Social Investment Principle (SIP; Roberts, Wood, & Smith, 2005), holds that transitions to, and increased investment in normative roles of adult social life (e.g., starting a career, a serious relationship, or a family) predict changes in agreeableness, conscientiousness, and emotional stability. Finally, it has been argued that the aforementioned three traits are all related to the serotonergic neurobiological system (DeYoung and Gray, 2009, DeYoung et al., 2002).

There are also theoretical grounds to assume that changes in extraversion and openness would be related to one another. Specifically, Digman (1997) proposed associations among these traits because both traits have linkages with the broad concept of personal growth and self-actualization. Personal growth involves exploration, which, in turn, is linked to the dopaminergic neurobiological system. Hence, along the same lines, DeYoung et al., 2002, DeYoung and Gray, 2009) proposed that the dopaminergic system might simultaneously affect extraversion and openness.

Neurobiological systems such as the dopaminergic system are known to change throughout the lifespan (e.g., Weickert et al., 2007). If the abovementioned theoretical ideas would hold, then changes in, for example, the dopaminergic system should affect levels of extraversion and openness simultaneously producing correlated change between these two traits. Similarly, mechanisms affecting agreeableness, conscientiousness, and emotional stability simultaneously should lead to correlated change between these three traits. Thus, there are several reasons to expect correlated change in general, with the strength of the correlated change being dependent on the particular trait pair that is considered.

There already is some research that has addressed the concept of correlated change. The first of these studies (Allemand et al., 2007) focused on cohorts of middle-aged individuals (i.e., 42–46 years of age) and older individuals (i.e., 60–64 years of age). In both cohorts, there was a considerable amount of correlated change across a four-year period. In particular, changes in emotional stability were strongly associated with changes in extraversion and conscientiousness, and changes in extraversion and conscientiousness were also strongly associated with one another. The overall amount of correlated change appeared to be about equal in the two age cohorts.

The older cohort was reassessed 8 years later, which allowed Allemand et al. (2008) to examine correlated change in older individuals across a 12-year period. Interestingly, this follow-up yielded quite different results, despite that the same analyses were applied. That is, changes in emotional stability were no longer significantly associated with changes in other Big Five traits. However, changes in all other Big Five traits were substantially associated with one another. Especially the amount of correlated change of conscientiousness with extraversion and agreeableness was considerable, with coefficients larger than .60. The fact that the patterns of correlated change observed in the same cohort across 4 years did not replicate across 12 years might suggest that the interval between measurement occasions plays a role.

Adding to the inconsistency observed in previous studies, a recent third study examining correlated change between ages 21 and 61 (Soto & John, 2012) found no evidence for correlated change in Big Five traits at all. However, this study employed a small non-representative sample exclusively consisting of women who graduated from college in the 1960s. As a result of the multiple differences in sample composition, the discrepancies between the amounts of correlated change reported by Soto and John, 2012, Allemand et al., 2007, Allemand et al., 2008 might be due to age differences, gender differences, and differences in educational level between samples. Furthermore, the possibility that broad mechanisms may lead to greater amounts of correlated change in some trait pairs than in others has not been formally tested. Finally, and perhaps most importantly, the inconsistency in the findings of previous studies suggests that it may also be warranted to examine possible moderators of correlated change.

A comparison of the samples that were employed in previous studies on correlated change already provides some leads of what factors may have contributed to their inconsistent results. First, the samples were differently aged. There are theoretical reasons for why age might be an important moderator of correlated change and may hence have contributed to inconsistency in findings across previous studies. That is, the aforementioned developmental mechanisms may be more active and neurobiological systems may undergo more changes at particular points in the lifespan. In this context, it may already be worth mentioning that the previously discussed SIP mainly applies to young adults (Roberts et al., 2005). This is because the transitions to and increases in investment in social roles of adult life, which are thought to cause increases in agreeableness, conscientiousness, and emotional stability, mainly take place in young adulthood (Roberts et al., 2005). Furthermore, there generally is more change in the structure of neural systems, and hence likely also more change in neurobiological systems, in adolescence and late adulthood/old age than in early and middle adulthood (Giedd et al., 1999, Ziegler et al., 2012). Thus, if broadly acting mechanisms such as the SIP and neurobiological systems are indeed responsible for producing correlated change, the strength of correlated change should mirror developmental trends in these systems. In that case, it is likely that there are curvilinear age-related patterns of correlated change with the highest levels in the period from adolescence to young adulthood and in late adulthood.

Furthermore, developmental processes in personality are often thought to be related or even partly attributable to changes in cognitive ability. For instance, the finding that trait differentiation among the Big Five is low in adolescence, increases towards middle adulthood, and increases again from late adulthood onwards, has often been explained as a consequence of developmental increases in general cognitive ability and verbal comprehension (Allik et al., 2004, Soto et al., 2008). Indeed, several studies have reported more trait differentiation in individuals with high cognitive abilities versus individuals with lower cognitive abilities (Austin et al., 2002, Bowler et al., 2009, Di Blas and Carraro, 2011, Mottus et al., 2007, Toomela, 2003). Yet, there is also a number of studies (Austin et al., 1997, Mottus et al., 2007, Rammstedt et al., 2010) that found no evidence for associations of cognitive abilities with trait differentiation.

Though some findings suggest that cognitive ability may play a role in the structural links among traits, it is yet unclear whether possible age-related patterns in associations between changes in traits (i.e., correlated change) can also be explained by differences in cognitive abilities. The present study provides a first attempt to examine whether there are any effects of age on correlated change and, if so, whether these age effects are consistent with differences in cognitive abilities.

The primary goal of the present research is to obtain greater insight into correlated change among Big Five traits. In two studies, we will examine the extent to which there is evidence for correlated change. In addition, we will examine whether correlated change among trait pairs that have been linked to the same developmental processes (Agreeableness-Conscientiousness-Emotional Stability, and Extraversion-Openness) is greater than among trait pairs of which the traits have been linked to different processes. Specific features of the two studies allow us to examine the role of two potential moderators of correlated change. Specifically, Study 1 employs a large nationally representative German dataset (N = 14,886) that covered ages 17–96, enabling us to examine the effects of age on correlated change. Study 2 examined the effects of verbal and non-verbal cognitive ability on correlated change in a sample of German adolescents (N = 174). Specific hypotheses regarding both studies will be provided in the introduction to each of these studies.

Section snippets

Study 1: correlated change across the lifespan

Study 1 investigated age differences in correlated change in a large and representative sample (N = 14,886), covering the entire adult lifespan. This way, we were able to examine how much evidence there was for correlated change in general. In addition, the wide age range allowed us to directly examine age effects on patterns of correlated change. By using a representative design, we could further be more confident that results generalize to the broader population.

Due to inconsistency in their

Participants and procedure

Analyses were based on the German Socio-Economic Panel (SOEP). The SOEP contains an ongoing longitudinal representative large-scale assessment of private households in Germany since 1984. Information was gathered through interviews or via questionnaires in 2005 and 2009. Only the 14,886 individuals with information on at least 3 of the Big Five scales at each of the two measurement occasions as well as information on age and gender were included. We divided participants into fourteen cohorts

Results

We ran 10 multigroup cross-lagged panel models in Mplus 7 (Muthén & Muthén, 2012) to examine correlated change simultaneously in the aforementioned nine age cohorts. Each model contained two consecutive measurement occasions of two Big Five traits, in such a way that each Big Five trait was paired with each of the other four Big Five traits, resulting in a total of 10 models. The estimated models (see Fig. 1) contained the stability paths of Big Five traits, initial (i.e., Time 1) correlations,

Discussion of Study 1

Study 1 examined age differences in correlated change in a large and representative sample. Our results clearly show that there is evidence for correlated change in all of the considered age groups. However, it should be noted that correlated change coefficients are generally rather modest (i.e., <.15) at most stages in the lifespan. Thus, it seems that personality change is partly, but by no means entirely, driven by broad mechanisms affecting multiple traits.

Overall, the pattern of correlated

Study 2: effects of cognitive ability

As stated, age differences in correlated change may be a function of developmental changes in cognitive ability. For example, one might gain a more differentiated view of one’s changing personality once cognitive ability increases. Study 2 set out to test this possibility in a sample of adolescents.

During early adolescence, individuals are developing the cognitive maturity that is necessary for fine-grained and differentiated personality self-descriptions (Soto et al., 2008). Accordingly,

Participants and procedure

Participants were part of the Munich Longitudinal Study on the Genesis of Individual Competencies (LOGIC). The sample originally consisted of 230 children (51.7% boys) who were studied annually from their first or second year in preschool until age 12. Because the schools were selected from a broad spectrum of neighborhoods, the sample was relatively unbiased. Over 90% of the approached parents gave their consent for their child’s participation. Until age 12, attrition was low (19% over 8 years)

Results

To examine the impact of cognitive abilities on correlated change among Big Five traits, we first performed a median split procedure on adolescents verbal and performance intelligence quotients (IQs). For verbal IQ, the group with a high IQ (M = 119.92; SD = 8.31) consisted of 86 adolescents, whereas the group with an average IQ (M = 99.33; SD = 6.87) consisted of 88 adolescents. With regard to performance IQ, the high group (M = 125.44; SD = 9.08) consisted of 92 adolescents, whereas the average group (M = 

Discussion of Study 2

Study 2 supported the assumption that correlated change varies as a function of an individual’s cognitive ability. Moreover, this appears to be the case for both verbal and performance ability. Therefore, it seems reasonable to conclude that during adolescence, an age during which emerging cognitive abilities may still constitute a bottleneck for trait comprehension, correlated change may partly be a result of general cognitive limitations.

It is important to note that results from Study 2 are

General discussion

In order to understand developmental processes in personality across the lifespan, it is important to examine to what extent these processes are driven by mechanisms affecting multiple traits at the same time and narrowly operating mechanisms affecting single traits. The first position would imply substantial correlated change among all or sets of Big Five personality traits, whereas the second position would imply that changes in Big Five traits are independent from one another. Previous

References (42)

  • M. Allemand et al.

    Long-term correlated change in personality traits in old age

    Psychology and Aging

    (2008)
  • J. Allik et al.

    Personality development from 12 to 18 years of age: Changes in mean-levels and structure of traits

    European Journal of Personality

    (2004)
  • J.B. Asendorpf et al.

    Resilient, overcontrolled, and undercontrolled personality prototypes in childhood: Replicability, predictive power, and the trait-type issue

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1999)
  • Bleidorn, W., Klimstra, T. A., Denissen, J. J. A., Rentfrow, P. J., Potter, J., & Gosling, S. D. (2013). Personality...
  • K. Bollen

    Structural equations with latent variables

    (1989)
  • J.L. Bolton et al.

    Associations between polymorphisms of the dopamine receptor D2 and catechol-o-methyl transferase genes and cognitive function

    Behavioral Genetics

    (2010)
  • M. Dehne et al.

    Persönlichkeitsmerkmale im Sozio-oekonomischen Panel (SOEP) – Konzept, Umsetzung und empirische Eigenschaften

    Research Notes

    (2007)
  • Denissen, J. J. A., van Aken, M. A. G., & Roberts, B. W. (2011). Personality development across the life span. In T....
  • C.G. DeYoung et al.

    Personality neuroscience: Explaining individual differences in affect, behavior, and cognition

  • L. Di Blas et al.

    Relations between nonverbal intelligence and personality reports in late childhood

    Journal of Research in Personality

    (2011)
  • J.M. Digman

    Higher-order factors of the Big Five

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1997)
  • Cited by (54)

    • Are changes in the perception of major life events associated with changes in subjective well-being?

      2023, Journal of Research in Personality
      Citation Excerpt :

      The effect sizes for the statistically significant correlated changes between the perceived event characteristics and SWB were at least of medium size (0.19 ≤ rΔΔ ≤ 0.34; Funder & Ozer, 2019). Compared to results on correlated changes from other domains, the associations were about as strong as correlated changes among certain personality trait domains (Allemand & Martin, 2016; Klimstra et al., 2013) as well as correlated changes between positive affect and symptoms of anxiety (Hou et al., 2015). Taken together, Study 1 suggests that there are significant correlated changes between perceived event characteristics and SWB.

    • The unfolding dark side: Age trends in dark personality features

      2020, Journal of Research in Personality
      Citation Excerpt :

      In other words, longitudinal data can be used to estimate correlated change. Correlated change estimates have previously been interpreted as evidence for two dimensions sharing a common cause, or being part of the same broader construct (e.g., Allemand & Martin, 2016; Klimstra, Bleidorn, Asendorpf, van Aken, & Denissen, 2013). Thus, the absence or presence of correlated change between agreeableness and the Dirty Dozen and its scales may provide some insight into the Dirty Dozen’s incremental value over agreeableness in a much more direct manner than is possible with comparisons of cross-sectional age trends.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text