Elsevier

Journal of Psychiatric Research

Volume 58, November 2014, Pages 89-95
Journal of Psychiatric Research

Age effects on the default mode and control networks in typically developing children

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2014.07.004Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Investigation of the neurodevelopmental formation of the Default Mode and Control Networks.

  • The sample is from a population of a developing country with high genetic admixture.

  • Two MRI systems from the same manufacturer using the same parameters.

  • Our findings suggest an increase in the antero-posterior connectivity in both studied networks.

  • Graph analyses pointed out an increase in centrality with age.

Abstract

Background

The investigation of neurodevelopment during late childhood and pre-adolescence has recently attracted a great deal of interest in the field of neuroimaging. One promising topic in this field is the formation of brain networks in healthy subjects. The integration between neural modules characterizes the ability of the network to process information globally. Although many fMRI-based neurodevelopment studies can be found in the literature, the analyses of very large samples (on the order of hundreds of subjects) that focus on the late childhood/pre-adolescence period and resting state fMRI are scarce, and most studies have focused solely on North American and European populations.

Aims

In this study, we present a descriptive investigation of the developmental formation of the Default Mode Network and the Control Network based on a Brazilian, cross-sectional community sample of 447 typically developing subjects aged 7–15 years old.

Methods

Resting state fMRI data were acquired using two MRI systems from the same manufacturer using the same acquisition parameters. We estimated the age effects on the strength of the links (between brain regions) and the network features (graph descriptors: degree and eigenvector centrality).

Results

Our findings showed an increase in the antero-posterior connectivity in both studied networks during brain development. The graph analyses showed an increase in centrality with age for most regions in the Default Mode Network and the dorsal anterior and posterior cingulate, the right anterior insula and the left posterior temporal cortex in the Control Network.

Conclusion

We conclude that the period of 7–15 years of age is crucial for the development of both the Default Mode and Control networks, with integration between the posterior and anterior neuronal modules and an increase in the centrality measures of the hub regions.

Introduction

At rest, the brain displays patterns of spontaneous activity that form multiple dissociable networks that are increasingly thought to be related to a specific set of brain operations (Crossley et al., 2012). Two networks have been more consistently studied in the literature: the Default Mode Network (DMN) (Gusnard et al., 2001, Raichle et al., 2001) and the Control Network (CN) (Dosenbach et al., 2007, Dosenbach et al., 2006, Fair et al., 2007). The integrity of the brain operations that result from the activities of these networks is a direct consequence of their structural and functional development (Power et al., 2010). This relationship opens the possibility of predicting the level of brain maturity by examining the functional networks (Dosenbach et al., 2010) and will shed light on atypical development, which may be associated with mental health disorders (Insel, 2009).

The default mode network (DMN, see Supplementary Table 1 for a description of the regions) is a distributed network of brain regions that are more active during rest than during the performance of a task (Shulman et al., 1997a, Shulman et al., 1997b, Gusnard et al., 2001, Raichle et al., 2001). Several studies in healthy adults have provided evidence of task-induced decreases in brain activity relative to rest in this network (Binder et al., 1999, Mazoyer et al., 2001, McKiernan et al., 2003), with some authors suggesting that the DMN supports “self-referential” or “introspective” mental activity (Gusnard et al., 2001). Our knowledge of the DMN in children remains very limited. Some studies have not identified the default-mode-like components (Fransson et al., 2011, Fransson et al., 2007) in infants (40–44 weeks, acquisition in sleep) with very low gestational age (24–27 weeks) or full term (mean age at MRI acquisition was 39 weeks and 2 days). In addition, Smyser et al. (2010) completed a longitudinal study of preterm infants (initial scan at approximately 2 weeks old) and found a poorly assembled network. Others studies have provided some support for a rudimentary DMN assembly very early in life using different methods of analysis, including seed-based (Fair et al., 2008, Kelly et al., 2009), independent component analysis (Gao et al., 2009, Stevens et al., 2009, Supekar and Musen, 2009, Thomason et al., 2008) and graph theory approaches (Fair et al., 2008). In a cross-sectional groups comparison, Fair et al. (2008) described that the DMN is only sparsely connected in children (7–9 years), which is in contrast to adults (21–31 years), who present a highly integrated DMN (Fair et al., 2008). Current evidence supports an increase in the functional connectivity within the regions in the DMN with age (for a detailed review, see Power et al., 2010). However, the specific brain regions that are more sensitive to the age-related changes during childhood remain unclear. These previous studies point toward a crucial role of the anterior medial prefrontal cortex (aMPFC) and the posterior cingulate (and the precuneus) as network hubs. The literature also suggests the maturation of the prefrontal cortex during childhood and adolescence (Shaw et al., 2008). Thus, an increasing involvement of the aMPFC and the posterior cingulate through age is expected.

In contrast with the DMN, the control network (CN, see Supplementary Table 1 for regions description) is thought to be responsible for a task-dependent cognitive state mode, i.e., the general set that is maintained for the execution of goal-directed brain operations (Logan and Gordon, 2001). Dosenbach et al. showed that a network composed of the fronto-parietal and cingulo-opercular components presented activations across several tasks (Dosenbach et al., 2006, Dosenbach et al., 2007). The authors suggested that these regions formed a “core” task set system that was anatomically separate from the domain-specific task-set functions (Dosenbach et al., 2006). Only a few studies have explored the developmental changes in the control networks during childhood. In a cross-sectional study, Fair et al. (2007) showed that the CN undergoes increased functional segregation (decreased short-range connections) and integration (increased long-range connections) during childhood (7–15 years). In their study, the authors found a segregation of the dACC/msFC region from the frontoparietal network and a disconnection between the anterior and dorsolateral parts of the prefrontal cortex. Also in their study, by using a larger sample (7–35 years old), connections between the dorsal part of the anterior cingulate and the cingulo-opercular network, the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex to the left intraparietal sulcus (IPS) and the left frontal to the left IPS were positively correlated with age. These findings were reinforced by a group comparison between adults (21–31 years) and children (7–9 years). To summarize, most connections with strength that were negatively correlated with age were short-range connections, and the connections with positive correlations were long-range connections.

While motion can bias the estimate of the functional connectivity (Power et al., 2011, van Dijk et al., 2012, Yan et al., 2013, Power et al., 2014), these changes in long-range connections are consistently found in studies that adequately control for movement (Satterthwaite et al., 2013). Further studies are needed to understand how these networks are assembled in childhood and adolescence.

Nevertheless, most studies have focused on North American and European populations (Satterthwaite et al., 2014). Although some samples were heterogeneous (e.g., Caucasians and African Americans; Satterthwaite et al., 2014), they were restrictive not only in terms of the genetic diversity (miscegenation), but arguably also in the social and environmental conditions typical of the developed world. Second, the majority of the studies to date have included very specific sample ascertainment, i.e., voluntary research subjects or specific populations. Therefore, how these networks develop in non-referred children drawn from the community (i.e., the subjects were not referred from a hospital or clinic, but were recruited at school or home) remains to be further investigated. Moreover, past studies have relied on relatively small sample sizes and/or very broad age ranges, thereby limiting the ability to detect more subtle changes that occur in specific windows of development.

Finally, very few studies have used modern analytic approaches, such as graph descriptors, to investigate the developmental changes in the brain networks. Graph descriptors are suitable to explore the networks beyond the analysis of the strength in connectivity between pair nodes. The graph analysis of fMRI data usually considers the brain regions as nodes, and the edges that connect these nodes are the functional connectivity measures (e.g., the correlation between the BOLD signal at each region). One of the attractive features of this approach is the ability to quantify how central a node (centrality measures) is to the network from both the local and global perspectives (Rubinov and Sporns, 2010).

This study aimed to replicate previous findings of DMN and CN maturation in a cross-sectional sample of 447 healthy children (7–15 years) from a developing country with high genetic admixture. The subjects included in the study were carefully evaluated for the exclusion of mental disorders. The brain scans were performed at two research centers using the same acquisition parameters. The aim of this study was to investigate the functional connectivity and the graph centrality measures of the DMN and the CN in different aged youth. Although this was primarily a descriptive study, we hypothesized that the functional connectivity within the DMN and the CN, as well as the degree of centrality (integration from a more local perspective) and the EVC (global information processing), would present an increasing connectivity pattern across age, thereby indicating the formation of both networks during childhood.

Section snippets

Participants

This study is based on a subsample of the ‘High Risk Cohort Study for Psychiatric Disorders in Childhood’ (HRC), which included two centers in two Brazilian cities, São Paulo and Porto Alegre. Children from 22 public schools in the city of Porto Alegre and from 35 schools in São Paulo participated in a screening procedure. Data from 9937 interviews based on the Family History Survey (FHS) were collected. This sample was then subsampled into two subgroups: a random stratum and a high-risk

ROI-based correlations

Fourteen connections of the DMN network were identified as changing according to age (Fig. 2, links are highlighted in blue). The anterior medial prefrontal cortex (amPFC) was the region that showed the greatest number of connections correlated with age in this network. The estimates of the age effects on the regional-pairwise connectivity strength of the DMN are presented in the scatter-plots in Supplementary Fig. 2. The regression adjusted-R (r) coefficient and the age effects corrected p

Discussion

In this study, we demonstrated the differences in the connectivity within the regions of the DMN and the CN in subjects at different developmental stages, which ranged from childhood to early adolescence, thus providing further evidence of the formation of these networks.

Furthermore, as hypothesized, the age effects on the connectivity suggest that this developmental period may be a critical assembly point of the human DMN. We found that i) the anterior–posterior connections (predominately

Disclosure

Dr. Luis Augusto Rohde LAR has been on the speakers' bureau/advisory board and/or acted as consultant for Eli-Lilly, Janssen-Cilag, Novartis and Shire in the last three years. The ADHD and Juvenile Bipolar Disorder Outpatient Programs chaired by him received unrestricted educational and research support from the following pharmaceutical companies in the last three years: Eli-Lilly, Janssen-Cilag, Novartis, and Shire. He receives authorship royalties from Oxford Press and ArtMed. Dr. Rodrigo

Role of funding source

The opinions, hypothesis, conclusions and recommendations of this study are under the responsibility of the authors, which not necessary represents the opinion of the funding agencies.

Contributors

EAJ, AL, PM, LAR, ECM, RAF, APJ designed the study and wrote the protocol. GS, AG, FAP, PMP, AZ, MQH, MA, LMM, MAGD collected the data. JRS, GS, AG, FAP, PMP, GV, AZ, PM, NC, AL, LAR, ECM, RAB and APJ wrote the manuscript. JRS and GV undertook the statistical analysis. All authors contributed to the conceptual planning and discussions and have approved the final manuscript.

Acknowledgments

The authors are grateful to Sao Paulo Research Foundation – FAPESP (HRC grant 573974/2008-0, J.R.S grants 2013/10498-6 and 2013/00506-1), CAPES and CNPq, Brazil for funding this research. N.C. is supported by the Wellcome Trust.

References (42)

  • T.D. Satterthwaite et al.

    Heterogeneous impact of motion on fundamental patterns of developmental changes in functional connectivity during youth

    NeuroImage

    (2013)
  • K.S. Scherf et al.

    The amygdala: an agent of change in adolescent neural networks

    Horm Behav

    (2013)
  • M.E. Thomason et al.

    Default-mode function and task-induced deactivation have overlapping brain substrates in children

    NeuroImage

    (2008)
  • K.R.A. van Dijk et al.

    The influence of head motion on intrinsic functional connectivity MRI

    NeuroImage

    (2012)
  • M.W. Woolrich et al.

    Bayesian analysis of neuroimaging data in FSL

    NeuroImage

    (2009)
  • C.-G. Yan et al.

    A comprehensive assessment of regional variation in the impact of head micromovements on functional connectomics

    NeuroImage

    (2013)
  • J.R. Binder et al.

    Conceptual processing during the conscious resting state. A functional MRI study

    J Cogn Neurosci

    (1999)
  • P. Bonacich

    Power and centrality: a family of measures

    Am J Sociol

    (1987)
  • N.A. Crossley et al.

    Neurophysiological alterations in the prepsychotic phases

    Curr Pharm Des

    (2012)
  • N.U.F. Dosenbach et al.

    Distinct brain networks for adaptive and stable task control in humans

    Proc Natl Acad Sci USA

    (2007)
  • N.U.F. Dosenbach et al.

    Prediction of individual brain maturity using fMRI supplemental material

    Science

    (2010)
  • Cited by (68)

    • Theory of mind performance and prefrontal connectivity in adolescents at clinical high risk for psychosis

      2021, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience
      Citation Excerpt :

      We indeed found a positive association between connectivity and age in healthy volunteers in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex. In fact, the medial prefrontal cortex is thought to play a key role in the development of the DMN during adolescence, experiencing a sharp increase in connectivity during this period in healthy volunteers (Cai et al., 2018; Dosenbach et al., 2010; Mak et al., 2017; Sato et al., 2014; Truelove-Hill et al., 2020). A meta-analysis reporting greater connectivity within the DMN in adult samples relative to children and adolescents (Mak et al., 2017) suggested that methodological differences between studies could explain discrepant findings in the literature: those using independent component analysis and seed-based analysis tended to find positive correlations with age (Dosenbach et al., 2010; Sato et al., 2014), while those using a ROI-to-ROI approach tended to find negative correlations with age (Marek et al., 2015).

    • Children with euthymic bipolar disorder during an emotional go/nogo task: Insights into the neural circuits of cognitive-emotional regulation

      2021, Journal of Affective Disorders
      Citation Excerpt :

      Higher cognitive regions undergo long periods of postpartum development (Supekar et al., 2009; Vogel et al., 2010). The separation between executive control networks and salience networks and the integration within networks runs throughout the development of children and adolescents (Knyazev et al., 2017; Sato et al., 2014; Gao et al., 2015). The developing function of the two networks is consistent with parents' perception of their children's impulsivity (Davis et al., 2013; Inuggi et al., 2014).

    • Development of the default-mode network during childhood and adolescence: A longitudinal resting-state fMRI study

      2021, NeuroImage
      Citation Excerpt :

      Considering that the DMN is closely related to internal mentation activity and social domain functions (Blakemore, 2008; Buckner and DiNicola, 2019; Raichle, 2015; Spreng et al., 2009), exploring the development of the functional organization of the DMN during childhood and adolescence will shed light on the neural mechanisms supporting individual cognitive development. Several studies have investigated how the functional connectivity within the DMN develops in different age groups (de Bie et al., 2012; Fair et al., 2008; Rebello et al., 2018; Sato et al., 2014; Sherman et al., 2014; Xiao et al., 2016). The connectivity strength within the DMN is greater in older children than in younger children during childhood and adolescence (de Bie et al., 2012; Fair et al., 2008; Rebello et al., 2018; Sato et al., 2014; Sherman et al., 2014; Xiao et al., 2016) and is greater in adults than in children (Fair et al., 2008).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text