What can MEG neuroimaging tell us about reading?

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2008.12.004Get rights and content

Abstract

Learning to read is one of the most cognitively complex tasks we will ever learn to do. Thus understanding the reading process is not just intrinsically interesting, but can give us a number of valuable insights into the relationship between brain processes and cognitive behaviour. MEG neuroimaging allows us to investigate reading processes in terms of the spatial extent of cortical activations when reading, the timing between brain locations, and the frequency dynamics between different cortical areas. The big challenge now for neuroscience is to model all three components of neural behaviour in order to be able to really understand the complexity of human cognition.

Section snippets

What is MEG?

Magnetoencephalography (MEG) is a neuroimaging technique that measures the magnetic field patterns generated by the brain. MEG measures activity primarily from the post-synaptic potentials of pyramidal cortical cells. When large populations of neurons fire together, as is the case when the brain responds to some sensory input, then an electric current is measurable outside the head. This is the basis of the well-known EEG technique. The neuromagnetic correlate of the electric current measured

Spatial dynamics of reading

There are a number of excellent reviews that have mapped the functional components of the reading network (e.g., Fiez and Petersen, 1998, Jobard et al., 2003, Price, 2000, Pugh et al., 2001). The strength of MEG lies more in being able to combine spatial and temporal dynamics, so this section will serve more as a brief site-map to the more important components of the reading network.

Neuroimaging research has demonstrated a wide and complex network of cortical sites that are recruited in

Temporal dynamics of reading

Emerging MEG studies have allowed us to put tentative ‘time-tags’ on the areas of interest in the reading network, generally describing a posterior to anterior, and inferior to superior flow of activation. Earliest, pre-lexical activity occurs in the occipito-temporal areas (Pammer et al., 2004, Tarkiainen et al., 1999, Tarkiainen et al., 2002). Tarkiainen et al. (1999) showed distinct early components of the reading network within the first 200 ms, namely a midline occipital component (dark

Frequency dynamics – the new frontier

The reading network involves a number of spatially disparate sites and acquiring reading skills means that the brain must learn to communicate between these different sites, binding relevant information to access a coherent lexical entry and maintaining a smooth flow of dynamic processing. An emerging theme in reading research is the role of cortical connectivity in the neural networks that underlie skilled reading. MEG research in this area is still in its early stages and there are as yet a

What can MEG neuroimaging NOT tell us about reading?

One of the limitations of MEG is that accuracy in detecting sources decreases with increasing distance from the source (Hillebrand & Barnes, 2002). In other words, deeper sources are difficult to pick up and to localise accurately in MEG. This makes it more difficult to analyse contributions from deeper sources such as the thalamus for example. The thalamus has been consistently implicated in fMRI studies of reading and word recognition (e.g., Brunswick et al., 1999, Rumsey et al., 1997, Rumsey

Conclusion

In order to read fluently, the brain is required to recruit disparate cortical areas to function in a highly complex and dynamic way. Because MEG is sensitive to the spatial and temporal properties of cortical signals, it can provide a valuable contribution to the cortical processes involved in reading. MEG allows us to track the evolution of the time course of the information flow through the reading network, mapping the time course of the neuromagnetic signal to specific cortical locations.

References (99)

  • A. Hillebrand et al.

    A quantitative assessment of the sensitivity of whole-head MEG to activity in the adult human cortex

    Neuroimage

    (2002)
  • M. Hirata et al.

    Spatio-temporal distributions of brain oscillations during silent reading

  • M. Hirata et al.

    Determination of language dominance with synthetic aperture magnetometry. Comparison with the WADA test

    Neuroimage

    (2004)
  • R. Itier et al.

    Inversion and contrast reversal effects on face processing assessed by MEG

    Brain Research

    (2006)
  • W. Jermakowicz et al.

    Neural networks a century after Cajal

    Brain Research Reviews

    (2007)
  • G. Jobard et al.

    Evaluation of the dual route theory of reading; a metanalysis of 35 neuroimaging studies

    Neuroimage

    (2003)
  • M. Kutas et al.

    Event related potentials to semantically inappropriate and surprisingly large words

    Biological Psychology

    (1980)
  • V. Lamme et al.

    The distinct modes of vision offered by feedforward and recurrent processing

    Trends in Neuroscience

    (2000)
  • Q. Luo et al.

    Neural dynamics for facial threat processing as revealed by gamma band synchronization using MEG

    Neuroimage

    (2007)
  • K. Marinkovic et al.

    Spatiotemporal dynamics of modality-specific and supramodal word processing

    Neuron

    (2003)
  • G. McArthur et al.

    Speech and non-speech processing in people with specific language impairment: a behavioural and electrophysiological study

    Brain and Language

    (2005)
  • F. McNab et al.

    Semantic and phonological task-set priming and stimulus processing investigated using magnetoencephalography (MEG)

    Neuropsychologia

    (2007)
  • K. Pammer et al.

    Attentional shifting and the role of the dorsal pathway in visual word recognition

    Neuropsychologia

    (2006)
  • K. Pammer et al.

    Visual word recognition: the first half second

    Neuroimage

    (2004)
  • G. Pfurtscheller et al.

    Event-related EEG/MEG synchronization and desynchronization: basic principles

    Clinical Neurophysiology

    (1999)
  • G. Pfurtscheller et al.

    Post movement beta synchronisation: a correlate of an idling motor area?

    Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology

    (1996)
  • C. Price et al.

    The myth of the visual word form area

    Neuroimage

    (2003)
  • K. Pugh et al.

    Neurobiological studies of reading and reading disability

    Journal of Communication Disorders

    (2001)
  • L. Pylkkänen et al.

    Tracking the time course of word recognition with MEG

    Trends in Cognitive Sciences

    (2003)
  • L. Pylkkänen et al.

    Neuromagnetic evidence for the timing of lexical activation: an MEG component sensitive to phonotactic probability but not to neighbourhood density

    Brain and Language

    (2002)
  • P. Sauseng et al.

    What does phase information of oscillatory brain activity tell us about cognitive processes?

    Neuroscience and Biobehavioural Reviews

    (2008)
  • M. Shadlen et al.

    Synchrony unbound: a critical evaluation of the temporal binding hypothesis

    Neuron

    (1999)
  • S. Shaywitz et al.

    Neural systems for compensation and persistence: young adult outcome of childhood reading disability

    Biological Psychiatry

    (2003)
  • P. Simos et al.

    Brain activation profiles in dyslexic children during non-word reading: a magnetic source imaging study

    Neuroscience Letters

    (2000)
  • M. Tamhankar et al.

    Alexia without agraphia following biopsy of a left thalamic tumour

    Paediatric Neurology

    (2004)
  • S. Ukai et al.

    Parallel distributed processing in the Sroop task using spatially filtered magnetoencephalography

    Neuroscience Letters

    (2002)
  • T.R. Vidyasagar

    A neuronal model of attentional spotlight: parietal guiding the temporal

    Brain Research Reviews

    (1999)
  • J. Vrba

    Magnetoencephalography: the art of finding a needle in a haystack

    Physica C

    (2002)
  • J. Vrba et al.

    Signal processing in magnetoencephalography

    Methods

    (2001)
  • Attal, Y., Bhattacharjee, M., Yelnik, J., Cottereau, B., Lefévre, J., Okada, Y., et al. Modelling and detecting deep...
  • J. Barlow

    The electroencephalogram: Its patterns and origins

    (1993)
  • G. Barnes et al.

    Statistical flattening of MEG beamformer images

    Human Brain Mapping

    (2003)
  • G. Barnes et al.

    Realistic spatial sampling for MEG beamformer images

    Human Brain Imaging

    (2004)
  • H. Berger

    Über das Elektroenkephalogramm des Menschen

    Archiv für Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankheiten

    (1929)
  • N. Brunswick et al.

    Explicit and implicit processing of words and pseudowords by adult developmental dyslexics – a search for Wernicke's Wortschatz?

    Brain

    (1999)
  • L. Cohen et al.

    The visual word form area – spatial and temporal characterization of an initial stage of reading in normal subjects and posterior split-brain patients

    Brain

    (2000)
  • L. Cohen et al.

    Language-specific tuning of visual cortex functional properties of the Visual Word Form Area

    Brain

    (2002)
  • M. Coltheart et al.

    Serial processing in reading aloud – evidence for dual route models of reading

    Journal of Experimental Psychology, Human Perception and Performance

    (1994)
  • J. Daunizeau et al.

    Assessing the relevance of fMRI-based prior in the EEG inverse problem: a Bayesian model comparison approach

    IEEE Transactions on Signal Processing

    (1995)
  • Cited by (8)

    • The neuro-cognitive representations of symbols: the case of concrete words

      2017, Neuropsychologia
      Citation Excerpt :

      The two stages of symbolic processing, the identification and the semantic stage, can be dissociated not only in terms of content (see 1) and localization (see 2), but also with respect to their temporal dynamics. Overall, during single word reading, brain activation unfolds from occipital areas towards the anterior temporal pole (Marinkovic et al., 2003; Pammer, 2009). Similarly, listening first elicits activity in primary auditory areas and subsequently in supramodal temporal areas including the anterior temporal pole (Marinkovic et al., 2003; Salmelin, 2007).

    • Right semantic modulation of early MEG components during ambiguity resolution

      2013, NeuroImage
      Citation Excerpt :

      It is commonly viewed that the orthographic decoding is manifested early on in the N170 component (Bentin et al., 1999; Cohen et al., 2000). There is also agreement that the N400 (M350) component reflects semantic processing (see review: Pammer, 2009). However, some studies (Dell'Acqua et al., 2007; Pulvermüller et al., 2001; Sereno et al., 1998) have found evidence for semantic processing less than 200 ms after word onset.

    • The neural basis of the right visual field advantage in reading: An MEG analysis using virtual electrodes

      2011, Brain and Language
      Citation Excerpt :

      Responses in those regions appeared in the first time window of the active–passive comparison (0–200 ms), taking the form of increases in oscillatory power in the 1–10 Hz frequency band (Fig. 3). Central presentation of words has been observed to produce responses in the same extrastriate areas in PET and fMRI studies (Carreiras et al., 2007; Fiez & Petersen, 1998; Jobard et al., 2003; Mechelli et al., 2003; Pugh et al., 1996), as well as in MEG studies (Cornelissen et al., 2003, 2009; Dhond et al., 2001; Kujala et al., 2007; Marinkovic et al., 2003; Pammer et al., 2004; Salmelin et al., 2000; Tarkiainen et al., 1999; see Pammer, 2009, for a review). One possibility widely considered is that these areas are involved in extracting visual features from letters and other complex visual stimuli, and are therefore involved in an early stage in the process of visual word recognition (Salmelin, 2007).

    • Attention and the identification of parafoveal words in school-age children and adults

      2011, Journal of Neurolinguistics
      Citation Excerpt :

      On the other hand, our results cannot be explained solely by the evolution of an attentional system, because the children did not show a larger distractor effect when identifying object drawings. The attention system may act on the reading system as a “visual guidance system” (Pammer, 2009), which distributes attentional resources, searches for and selects different components of the text and prepares for expected information. Several studies have found a developmental increase in frontal and parietal cortex activity during attention-demanding reading-related tasks.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text