Secondary school students’ levels of understanding in computing exponents

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jmathb.2007.11.003Get rights and content

Abstract

The aim of this study is to describe and analyze students’ levels of understanding of exponents within the context of procedural and conceptual learning via the conceptual change and prototypes’ theory. The study was conducted with 202 secondary school students with the use of a questionnaire and semi-structured interviews. The results suggest that three levels of understanding can be identified. At the first level students’ interpretation of exponents is based upon exponents that symbolize natural numbers. At Level 2, students’ knowledge acquisition process is a process of enrichment of the existing conceptual structures. Students at this level are able to compute exponents with negative numbers by extending the application of prototype examples. Finally, at Level 3 students not only extend the prototype examples but also reorganize their thinking in order to compute and compare exponents with roots, a concept which is quite different from the concept of exponents with natural numbers.

Introduction

In recent years, there has been an increased interest in educational research in the understanding of advanced mathematics topics such as functions, limits, infinity (Dubinsky, 1991; Lakoff & Núñez, 2000; Sfard, 1991). However, there has been comparatively little research focused on students’ learning and understanding of exponents, which are important mathematical concepts and central to many collegiate mathematics courses, including calculus, differential equations and complex analysis. Much of the published research on this topic consists of researchers’ suggestions and descriptions of instructional designs to be used in teaching students about exponents (Barnes, 2006, Weber, 2002), while as Confrey and Smith (1995) note, there have been relatively few studies about students’ understanding of exponential numerals. Moreover, the research focusing directly on exponents as an autonomous mathematical object is very limited, with more studies considering exponents in conjunction with functions and concatenations (Confrey & Smith, 1995; Lee & Messner, 2000). In particular, little is known about students’ mental constructions and the way in which they develop a meaningful understanding of exponents or logarithms.This article deals with students’ understanding of exponents and is part of a large research and development project, initiated 5 years ago, that investigated a number of calculus concepts (Christou, Pitta-Pantazi, Souyoul, & Zachariades, 2005). The main purpose of this study is to describe the students’ levels understanding of exponents and to analyze students’ understanding within the context of procedural and conceptual learning via the conceptual change and prototypes’ theory.

Section snippets

Theoretical framework

In this section, we first refer to the approaches that have been developed to explain the mechanisms governing concept learning by focusing on the theory of conceptual change and the role of prototypes. Second, we discuss previous research on exponents.

Purpose of the study

The purpose of this study was to investigate the present situation concerning the knowledge of exponents by high school students in Cyprus. It has been found that the concept of exponents is not clearly defined in the students’ minds (Sastre & Mullet, 1998; Weber, 2002). As a matter of fact, the concept of exponents is taught mainly through a few examples with little attention being placed on the conceptual development of exponents.

In addition, research has shown that the concept of exponents

Participants

The sample of the study comprised 202 high school students. These students were approximately equally divided between boys and girls and came from middle class public schools. All students had some familiarity with the concept and notational conventions of exponential expressions because these were introduced in the curriculum 2 months prior the administration of the test. Specifically, during the instruction students initially assumed that the exponents were positive and looked at zero (a = 1)

Results

The results are presented within the context of prototypes and conceptual theory and according to the aim of the study which focuses on students’ understanding of exponents. To this end, many of the presented interviews excerpts purported to clarify the way in which students think. Much less attention was paid to their difficulties and misconceptions.

Discussion of the results—the proposed model

Based on the results of this study and the abilities exhibited by students in each group, a model of students’ exponential reasoning can be proposed. This model is presented in Table 3. In the first column of Table 3, we describe the levels of students’ progress as they develop their understanding of exponents. The levels are based on (a) students’ thinking while comparing exponential expression, and (b) the conceptual theory, the theory of conceptual change and the prototypes ideas of

Conclusions

Conceptual change (Merenluoto & Lehtinen, 2002), as the process through which students fundamentally expand, revise or change their existing knowledge, provides the means for tracing how students with initial knowledge of calculating simple exponential processes (such as ax where a and b are positive integers—the prototype concept) can arrive at a conceptual understanding of exponents involving not only real but irrational numbers as well. The understanding of exponent expressions is studied in

References (31)

  • E. Dubinsky

    Reflective abstraction in advanced mathematical thinking

  • N. Herscovics et al.

    A cognitive gap between arithmetic and algebra

    Educational Studies in Mathematics

    (1994)
  • R. Hershkowitz

    Visualization in geometry—Two sides of the coin

    Focus on Learning Problems in Mathematics

    (1989)
  • C. Kieran

    The learning and teaching of school algebra

  • G. Lakoff et al.

    Where mathematics comes from: How the embodied mind brings mathematics into being

    (2000)
  • Cited by (20)

    • High school student understanding of exponential and logarithmic functions

      2022, Journal of Mathematical Behavior
      Citation Excerpt :

      Indeed, Cangelosi et al. (2013) found that distinguishing between additive and multiplicative inverses, and thus negative exponents, accounted for most persistent errors in students' algebraic understanding of exponential expressions. Also, Pitta-Pantazi et al. (2007) underscore this difficulty in their model (of students’ reasoning in solving exponential tasks) with a change from the level where students make sense of positive integer powers to the next level where they extend their understanding to include exponents with negative numbers. Anne: … at first glance, I know that the number below [referring to ½] is an exponent, I don’t know, it’s like… I feel like looking at it as if it were a square root or something… I don’t know… as if it were a radical… [latter] I don’t know but if we have two identical cards the number above should be the same… but 2 is a prime number… no… so, I don’t know

    • Prospective teachers’ skills of attending, interpreting and responding to content-specific characteristics of mathematics instruction in classroom videos

      2020, Teaching and Teacher Education
      Citation Excerpt :

      For this purpose, edited class videos containing the instructional applications of two mathematics teachers on the same mathematics subject, specifically exponents, were used. The reason for focusing on exponents is that according to previous research, students encounter various obstacles in the teaching and learning process of this concept (e.g., Cangelosi, Madrid, Cooper, Olson, & Hartter, 2013; Pitta-Pantazi, Christou, & Zachariades, 2007; Ulusoy, 2019), and there are few studies on teachers’ mathematical knowledge for teaching exponents (e.g., Levenson, 2012). Hence, exponential expressions are an important mathematical concept to be considered about what alternative teaching practices there might be.

    • Undergraduates’ images of the root concept in ℝ and in ℂ

      2018, Journal of Mathematical Behavior
      Citation Excerpt :

      Accordingly, roots serve as a bridging concept between secondary and tertiary mathematics, where undergraduates’ understanding of roots is extended to new mathematical domains and extensively exploited for exemplifying more advanced ideas. According to the model of Pitta-Pantazi, Christou and Zachariades (2007), manipulations with roots are associated with the highest level of exponential reasoning. At the lowest level, students operate with natural exponents of integers.

    • Middle-school students' concept images of geometric translations

      2014, Journal of Mathematical Behavior
      Citation Excerpt :

      “During the mental processes of recalling and manipulating a concept, many associated processes are brought into play, consciously and unconsciously affecting the meaning and usage” (Tall & Vinner, 1981, p. 152). Several researchers (e.g., Schwarz & Hershkowitz, 1999; Tall & Bakar, 1992) suggested that students generally rely on special examples of mathematical concepts which are called “prototypes” when learning new concepts (Pitta-Pantazi, Christou, & Zachariades, 2007). Prototypes are mental representations (Lakoff, 1987) and part of concept images.

    • The negative sign and exponential expressions: Unveiling students' persistent errors and misconceptions

      2013, Journal of Mathematical Behavior
      Citation Excerpt :

      These exponential expressions contained numbers which were too large to calculate using pencil and paper; instead students had to rely on properties of exponents and their knowledge of number systems. Weber (2002a, 2002b) and Pitta-Pantazi et al. (2007) examined students’ conceptions about exponential expressions. Weber looked at post-secondary students’ thinking regarding such expressions in the context of APOS theory: as an action; as a process; and then as a mathematical object that is the result of a process.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text