An affirmed self and a better apology: The effect of self-affirmation on transgressors' responses to victims

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2014.04.013Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Tested whether self-affirmation could promote more effective apologies.

  • Affirmed transgressors included more apology elements and fewer defensive strategies.

  • Affirmed transgressors were thus more likely to respond in ways that boost forgiveness.

  • First empirical research to identify a method for promoting more effective apologies.

  • Successful application of self-affirmation theory to interpersonal conflict resolution.

Abstract

Comprehensive apologies are powerful tools that transgressors can use to promote reconciliation with the people they have hurt. However, because many apology elements require transgressors to admit fault, express shameful emotions and promise change, transgressors often avoid these threatening elements and instead choose to use more perfunctory apologies or even defensive strategies, such as justifications or attempts to blame the person they hurt. In two studies, I aimed to increase apology comprehensiveness and reduce defensiveness using self-affirmation. I predicted that self-affirmation would help transgressors maintain their self-integrity, consequently allowing them to offer more comprehensive apologies and bypass defensive strategies. Participants received a values affirmation, recalled an unresolved conflict, and indicated what they would say to the person they had hurt. As predicted, affirmed participants offered more comprehensive apologies and used fewer defensive strategies than control participants. These studies thus identify a simple method for promoting responses that facilitate conflict resolution and demonstrate the successful application of self-affirmation to the domain of interpersonal conflict.

Introduction

One of the unfortunate certainties of life is that we sometimes hurt people we care about. Luckily, these conflict events do not have to be detrimental to our relationships. Our relationship partners can forgive us for our harmful actions, and this forgiveness can increase their feelings of closeness (McCullough et al., 1998) and their willingness to cooperate and prioritize the needs of the relationship (Karremans & Van Lange, 2004). Moreover, actively discussing and working to resolve relationship problems are associated with positive feelings between partners, as well as both short- and long-term benefits to the relationship (Overall, Sibley, & Travaglia, 2010). Thus, when managed well, conflicts can be functional and contribute to positive relationship outcomes (Gottman and Krokoff, 1989, Markman et al., 1988).

When managed poorly, however, conflicts can be detrimental to relationship satisfaction, causing lasting resentment and even relationship dissolution (Carrere and Gottman, 1999, Cramer, 2000). These negative effects are not limited to romantic partnerships. Ongoing conflicts can harm other types of relationships (e.g., friendships: Raffaelli, 1997; family: Overall et al., 2010) and have consequences that extend beyond relationship outcomes. For example, unresolved conflict with a colleague in the workplace is associated with reduced organizational commitment, increased intentions to quit, and poor task performance (De Dreu and Weingart, 2003, Morrison, 2008). The ability to successfully manage and resolve interpersonal conflict thus has diverse implications for the discordant relationship, its individual members, and others in the broader social or work network.

Section snippets

Comprehensive apologies as tools for conflict resolution

In attempting to manage a conflict, the offending person (transgressor) can perform actions that influence whether the offended person (victim) will respond with forgiveness or continued anger and resentment. Research on conflict management suggests that an apology is one of the most powerful tools transgressors can use to promote reconciliation with the victim (Fehr, Gelfand, & Nag, 2010). Apologies increase victim forgiveness, reduce anger and aggression toward the transgressor, and validate

Barriers to offering comprehensive apologies

If comprehensive apologies are so effective at promoting reconciliation with the victim, why don't transgressors use them in every conflict situation? I propose that transgressors may avoid offering comprehensive apologies because it can be threatening to do so. People are highly motivated to maintain their sense of self-worth and integrity (Sherman & Cohen, 2006), but the act of harming another person can threaten one's identity as a good and appropriate person (Aronson, 1999, Goffman, 1971,

Self-affirmation as a means of promoting more effective apologies

Self-affirmation theory (Steele, 1988) posits that people can protect their self-integrity from threats by reflecting on other important values and sources of self-worth. Reflecting on core values allows people to adopt a more expansive view of the self, weakening the implications of a threat for their self-integrity. With their self-integrity intact, they can bypass defensive behaviors aimed at protecting the self from the threat (Cohen and Sherman, 2014, Sherman and Cohen, 2006). As a result,

Study 1

In Study 1, I conducted an initial test of my hypothesis by randomly assigning adult participants to either a traditional self-affirmation condition or a control condition and then having them write what they would say to a person they had hurt. I predicted that participants who had the opportunity to affirm core values would write responses that included more apology elements and fewer defensive strategies.

Study 2

In Study 2, I aimed to replicate and develop a deeper understanding of the effects observed in Study 1. First, I sought to rule out mood as an alternate cause of the effect of self-affirmation on transgressors' responses. Although most evidence suggests that mood cannot explain the effects of self-affirmation (e.g., Cohen et al., 2000, Sherman et al., 2000, Shrira and Martin, 2005), some studies have shown a positive effect of self-affirmation on mood (e.g., Koole, Smeets, van Knippenberg, &

General discussion

Conflict is an inevitable aspect of social interactions. But if managed well, hurtful events can be transformed into constructive experiences that might even improve the relationship between the transgressor and the victim. An impressive amount of scholarship has demonstrated the benefits of forgiveness and reconciliation (e.g., Karremans and Van Lange, 2004, Witvliet et al., 2001) and has identified processes—such as comprehensive apologies—that promote reconciliation (Fehr et al., 2010). The

References (64)

  • C.M. Steele

    The psychology of self-affirmation: Sustaining the integrity of the self

  • J.C. Anderson et al.

    Influence of apologies and trait hostility on recovery from anger

    Journal of Behavioral Medicine

    (2006)
  • E. Aronson

    Dissonance, hypocrisy, and the self-concept

    Readings about the social animal

    (1999)
  • M. Burhmester et al.

    Amazon's Mechanical Turk: A new source of inexpensive, yet high quality, data?

    Perspectives on Psychological Science

    (2011)
  • S. Carrere et al.

    Predicting divorce among newlyweds from the first three minutes of a marital conflict discussion

    Family Process

    (1999)
  • S. Cehajic-Clancy et al.

    Affirmation, acknowledgment of ingroup responsibility, group-based guilt, and support for reparative measures

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (2011)
  • G.L. Cohen et al.

    When beliefs yield to evidence: Reducing biased evaluation by affirming the self

    Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin

    (2000)
  • G.L. Cohen et al.

    Recursive processes in self-affirmation: Intervening to close the minority achievement gap

    Science

    (2009)
  • G.L. Cohen et al.

    The psychology of change: Self-affirmation and social psychological intervention

    Annual Review of Psychology

    (2014)
  • G.L. Cohen et al.

    Bridging the partisan divide: Self-affirmation reduces ideological closed-mindedness and inflexibility in negotiation

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (2007)
  • D. Cramer

    Relationship satisfaction and conflict style in romantic relationships

    The Journal of Psychology

    (2000)
  • C.R. Critcher et al.

    When self-affirmations reduce defensiveness: Timing is key

    Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin

    (2010)
  • J. Crocker et al.

    Why does writing about important values reduce defensiveness? Self-affirmation and the role of positive, other directed feelings

    Psychological Science

    (2008)
  • B.W. Darby et al.

    Children's reactions to apologies

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1982)
  • C.K.W. De Dreu et al.

    Task versus relationship conflict, team performance, and team member satisfaction: A meta-analysis

    Journal of Applied Psychology

    (2003)
  • J.A. Eaton

    Repentance as validation: Toward an understanding of the mechanisms behind repentance and forgiveness

    Dissertation Abstracts, Section B: The Sciences and Engineering

    (2006)
  • J.J. Exline et al.

    Is apology worth the risk? Predictors, outcomes, and ways to avoid regret

    Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology

    (2007)
  • R. Fehr et al.

    The road to forgiveness: A meta-analytic synthesis of its situational and dispositional correlates

    Psychological Bulletin

    (2010)
  • F.D. Fincham et al.

    Longitudinal relations between forgiveness and conflict resolution in marriage

    Journal of Family Psychology

    (2007)
  • E. Goffman

    Relations in public

    (1971)
  • J.M. Gottman et al.

    Marital interaction and satisfaction: A longitudinal view

    Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology

    (1989)
  • J. Gottman et al.

    The seven principles for making marriage work: A practical guide from the country's foremost relationship expert

    (1999)
  • J.H. Hall et al.

    The temporal course of self-forgiveness

    Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology

    (2008)
  • J. Holmes

    Apologies in New Zealand English

    Language in Society

    (1990)
  • R. Itoi et al.

    A cross-cultural study of preference of accounts: Relationship closeness, harm severity, and motives of account making

    Journal of Applied Social Psychology

    (1996)
  • J.C. Karremans et al.

    Back to caring after being hurt: The role of forgiveness

    European Journal of Social Psychology

    (2004)
  • J. Kirchhoff et al.

    Apologies: Words of magic? The role of verbal components, anger reduction, and offense severity

    Peace and Conflict: Journal of Peace Psychology

    (2012)
  • S.L. Koole et al.

    The cessation of rumination through self-affirmation

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1999)
  • K.A. Lawler et al.

    The unique effects of forgiveness on health: An exploration of pathways

    Journal of Behavioral Medicine

    (2005)
  • A. Lazare

    On apology

    (2004)
  • L. Legault et al.

    Preserving integrity in the face of performance threat: Self-affirmation enhances neurophysiological responsiveness to task errors

    Psychological Science

    (2012)
  • H.J. Markman et al.

    The prevention of marital distress: A longitudinal investigation

    Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology

    (1988)
  • Cited by (72)

    • Would you help me again? The role of moral identity, helping motivation and quality of gratitude expressions in future helping intentions

      2022, Personality and Individual Differences
      Citation Excerpt :

      Although we do not dispute this finding, we understand that the quality of what is communicated also matters. For instance, research on reconciliation showed that, more than apologizing, offenders should provide high-quality apologies to increase their chances of being forgiven by better communicating remorse, a desire to amend the situation and a promise of change (Ludwig et al., 2022; Schumann, 2014). We believe that the same idea of quality may apply to expressions of gratitude.

    • Humble and apologetic? Predicting apology quality with intellectual and general humility

      2022, Personality and Individual Differences
      Citation Excerpt :

      However, transgressors who feel little concern for the victim might feel less motivated to engage in this other-oriented behavior. Transgressors might also be reluctant to offer a high-quality apology because they believe that doing so will associate them with wrongful behavior—an association that threatens their self-image as a moral person who deserves positive social relationships (Schumann, 2014, 2018). Indeed, transgressors are often reluctant to apologize because they overestimate how humiliating and stressful it will feel (Leunissen et al., 2014).

    • Crisis communication: The mediating role of cognitive and affective empathy in the relationship between crisis type and crisis response strategy on post-crisis reputation and forgiveness

      2022, Public Relations Review
      Citation Excerpt :

      This calls for crafting press releases that could cultivate empathy among the publics and showing that the organization is doing all it can to prevent the recurrence of the crisis. Another way of inducing empathetic feelings is by actively listening to the victims, asking them to explain their perspectives, and consciously trying to share the victims’ affective states (Eyal, Steffel, & Epley, 2018; Schumann, 2014). Moreover, crisis spokespeople should examine the nature of the crisis before offering a response.

    View all citing articles on Scopus

    This article was prepared with the support of a Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada Postdoctoral Fellowship (award # 756-2011-0397) awarded to Karina Schumann.

    View full text