Children’s intergroup helping: The role of empathy and peer group norms

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2014.06.002Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Two studies examined children’s (8–13 years) intergroup helping intentions.

  • In a low need public helping context, children intend to help out-group peers more.

  • Public out-group helping in low need depends on the norm about the out-group.

  • When the need is high, empathy outweighs children’s group norm considerations.

  • Neither study showed developmental differences in children’s intention to help.

Abstract

Two studies examined children’s (8- to 13-year-olds) intergroup helping intentions. In Study 1, 856 children indicated their intention to help national in-group or out-group peers in a high need situation and in either a public or private context. Results showed that children’s empathic tendencies predicted their intention to help and that the context as well as recipients’ group membership had no effects. In Study 2, 388 children indicated their intention to help in-group and out-group peers in either a low need or high need situation. Results of Study 1 were replicated. In addition, in the low need situation and when helping was public, children intended to help out-group peers more than in-group peers, particularly when they perceived an accepting descriptive classroom norm about the out-group. When the need was relatively high, empathy appeared to outweigh children’s group norm considerations. In all analyses, no age differences were found.

Introduction

The human capacity to take care of others emerges early in life. Young children are capable of understanding another person’s need (Eisenberg, 1992); they often respond with empathy and prosociality (Eisenberg and Fabes, 1998, Thompson et al., 1997, Zahn-Waxler et al., 1992) and offer help already at 18 months of age (Warneken & Tomasello, 2006). Children have a tendency to empathize and help others in need (e.g., Eisenberg, 1992). Yet, prosociality has been predominantly studied in interpersonal contexts, and less is known about children’s intergroup helping in which ethnic, national, or other group boundaries are salient. Furthermore, whereas an increasing number of studies examine the role of social group norms in children’s negative intergroup attitudes (e.g., Abrams and Rutland, 2008, De França and Monteiro, 2013, Nesdale and Lawson, 2011), not much is known about the influence of these norms in intergroup helping situations.

The current experimental vignette research examines the roles of children’s (8- to 13-year-olds) empathic tendency and perceived group norms in an intergroup helping context. In two studies, Dutch children’s public or private intention to help Dutch in-group peers or German out-group peers was investigated. Study 1 examined helping intentions in relation to children’s empathy when the need for help was relatively high. Study 2 also assessed a low need situation in which concerns about social group norms are likely to emerge. These concerns were examined by manipulating help within a public or private classroom context and by assessing perceived classroom norms about the out-group. Below, we first discuss hypotheses pertaining to the role of empathy in children’s helping intentions (Studies 1 and 2). Next, we discuss expectations relating to the high need context (Study 1), followed by the hypotheses for the low need context (Study 2).

During primary school, children increasingly help others by sharing objects or money and assisting in emergency situations (Eisenberg, 1992, Radke-Yarrow et al., 1983). Numerous researchers have shown that prosocial behavior is related to children’s disposition to empathize with others (e.g., Eisenberg, 1992, Eisenberg et al., 2001, Eisenberg et al., 1987, Malti et al., 2009), and empathic children help more when there are clear cues indicating need and distress (e.g., Eisenberg, 1992, Gelfand et al., 1975, Li et al., 2013). Therefore, we expect that in general more empathic children will intend to help more.

Whereas empathy can be expected to influence children’s general intention to help others, additional considerations are likely to be important when children think about helping in-group or out-group members in a public or private context. Study 1 examined children’s intention to help when need is relatively high. Children aged 8 to 13 years consider it a moral obligation to help in high need situations (e.g., Sierksma, Thijs, Verkuyten, & Komter, 2014). Social cognitive domain theory (Turiel, 1983) argues that moral considerations are general, obligatory, and inalterable. From early childhood onward, children understand and apply moral principles in their reasoning about social behavior (Killen, Lee-Kim, McGlothlin, & Stangor, 2002) and do not differentiate among different recipients when need is high (Miller, Bersoff, & Harwood, 1990). High need situations tend to evoke moral concerns that are general and not context or target specific. In these situations, one is morally expected to offer help independent of whether others are present and whether it is an in-group or out-group member who needs help. Therefore, in the context of high need (Study 1), we expected that children’s intention to help would depend on their empathic disposition and not on the context of helping (i.e., public vs. private) or the group membership of the peer in need of help (i.e., in-group vs. out-group).

When need for help is less urgent, children not only may feel morally obliged to offer help but also may consider peer group norms. Therefore, in Study 2 we compared a low need situation with a high need situation and considered group norms. Children have a basic desire to be accepted and to belong (Baumeister & Leary, 1995), and peers are significant others who function as important sources for appropriate behavior (Killen et al., 2002, Smetana et al., 2009). Peer group norms about intergroup relations become salient around middle childhood (Killen, Rutland, Abrams, Mulvey, & Hitti, 2013) and affect children’s intergroup attitudes and behavioral intentions (Bigler et al., 1997, De França and Monteiro, 2013, Nesdale and Lawson, 2011). Moreover, children adjust their intergroup behavior to the specific in-group norm (FitzRoy & Rutland, 2010) and disapprove of others who do not do so (Abrams and Rutland, 2008, Killen and Stangor, 2001).

In a public context where accountability is relatively high, social norms become salient and promote self-presentational behavior (Rutland, Cameron, Milne, & McGeorge, 2005). From kindergarten onward, children are concerned about their social reputation and understand how impression management might influence behavior of others (e.g., Banerjee et al., 2010, Banerjee and Yuill, 1999, Hatch, 1987, Sluckin, 1981). Children as young as 5 years behave more generously when they know that others are aware of their behavior (Leimgruber et al., 2012, Sierksma et al., 2014b), and the presence of peers influences children’s spontaneous positive affective responses (e.g., Castelli & Tomelleri, 2008). This means that in public contexts and when the situation involves low need, children might help peers relatively more. Thus, for Study 2, we hypothesized that children would intend to help more in public circumstances compared with private circumstances.

However, helping might also be related to who the recipient of help is. Children tend to focus on and prefer their in-group (Nesdale, 2007), and there is a general tendency to be concerned about the welfare of fellow in-group members (Brewer, 2007). Although refusing to help an in-group member might invite disapproval by the social group, providing in-group help is common and, thus, not very noteworthy (Hopkins et al., 2007). In contrast, out-group helping is less common and tends to attract more attention. This means that helping out-group members might be more effective to present oneself in a positive way and to be socially accepted. Yet, it is likely that this depends on perceived out-group norms. Out-group helping should invite approval by one’s peer group when the peer group norm about the out-group is relatively positive and not when the norm is rather negative. Therefore, in Study 2, children’s perception of the descriptive classroom norm about the out-group was examined. Descriptive norms refer to what important others do and think (Cialdini, Kallgren, & Reno, 1991). We expected that children would help the out-group relatively more in a public setting where there was a positive descriptive norm about the out-group. In testing this prediction, we also considered children’s own attitude toward the German out-group. If children’s public helping intentions are guided by group norms, they should follow the descriptive peer norm and not their own out-group attitude.

In contrast to public helping, private helping should not depend on peer group norms. Therefore, when helping is a private matter and need is low, other motivations might be relevant in children’s consideration to help peers. Two contrasting predictions can be made. On the one hand, social identity research has shown that there is a general tendency to be concerned about the welfare of fellow in-group members (Brewer, 2007). Furthermore, social identity theory (Tajfel & Turner, 1979) argues that the motivation to maintain a positive group identity tends to generate intergroup biases in which the in-group is favored over a relevant out-group. Research has shown that in-group preference is already present in children as young as 3 years (e.g., Kowalski and Lo, 2001, Martin and Fabes, 2001) both in the minimal group paradigm (Nesdale, Maass, Griffiths, & Durkin, 2003) and in real groups (e.g., Elashi et al., 2010, Patterson and Bigler, 2006). If social identity concerns motivate children’s private helping in a low need situation, this means that children will intend to help the in-group more compared with the out-group.

On the other hand, research shows that fair treatment and equal distribution are central to children’s helping and sharing behavior (e.g., Fehr et al., 2008, Geraci and Surian, 2011, Moore, 2009, Schmidt and Sommerville, 2011, Shaw and Olson, 2012, Sloane et al., 2012, Warneken and Tomasello, 2006). This suggests that children are intrinsically motivated to take care of others’ well-being. This means that when need is low (Study 2) and helping is private, children will intend to help in- and out-group members equally.

As they grow older, children develop a better understanding of how groups work, and they increasingly consider what is socially acceptable when expressing particular attitudes and behaviors (Abrams et al., 2007, Killen and Rutland, 2011, Nesdale, 2007). Furthermore, children develop a growing tendency toward displaying behavior that puts them in the best possible light to significant others (e.g., Aloise-Young, 1993, Banerjee and Yuill, 1999). Moreover, older children are better able to weigh various aspects in their decision to help, whereas younger children tend to focus more on the needs of peers (Eisenberg et al., 1987, Sierksma et al., 2014c). This makes it plausible that, with age, children become more responsive to norms of their peer group, especially in a public context. Therefore, it is likely that when the need for help is low (Study 2), older children’s intention to help will reflect the norm of their peer group more strongly compared with younger children. Age differences are less likely in the high need condition (Studies 1 and 2) and in the private context when need is low (Study 2) because social group norms are expected to be less important in these situations.

Two studies were conducted on children’s public and private intentions to help in- and out-group peers. In the first study, we aimed to demonstrate children’s intention to help peers in a high need situation and expected that children’s empathic disposition would predict their intention to help. The second study examined children’s helping intentions when need was either high or low, and in the latter condition social group norm considerations were expected to emerge. This means that in the public context, children are expected to help out-group peers more compared with in-group peers, but only when they perceive a positive descriptive norm about the out-group. Furthermore, these group norm considerations might be especially important for older children compared with younger children.

For low need and private helping, two contrasting predictions were formulated. On the one hand, children might help the in-group more compared with the out-group in order to maintain a positive in-group identity (Tajfel & Turner, 1979). On the other hand, children might intend to help in-group and out-group peers equally because fairness is central to helping. In addition, private helping should not be related to the descriptive norm about the out-group.

These predictions were tested by examining native Dutch children’s helping intentions toward the German out-group and their Dutch in-group. Germany is the largest and most important neighboring country of The Netherlands with which children are familiar. Research has shown that Dutch children see Germans as a typical out-group and that they have mixed beliefs about Germans and rather neutral feelings toward them (Du Bois-Reymond, 1998, Verkuyten, 2001). Yet, and comparable to British children (Rutland, 1999), Dutch children tend to evaluate Germans less favorably than the Dutch in-group (Verkuyten, 2001). These findings indicate that Germany is a relevant and meaningful out-group for Dutch children.

Section snippets

Participants and procedure

A total of 882 children (52.8% girls and 47.2% boys) participated in the study. Children came from 21 schools in various parts of The Netherlands. Only children in Grades 4 to 6 (mean age = 10.71 years, SD = 0.99) were included, involving a total of 48 classrooms. All children indicated that they, as well as their parents, were of ethnic Dutch origin. Because some children did not describe why they liked being Dutch (see below) and some children did not indicate their intention to help, 831 children

Study 2

A second study was conducted with two goals. First, we also examined children’s intention to help in-group and out-group peers when the need was less urgent. We predicted that when the need was relatively low, peer group norms in addition to children’s empathic tendency would predict helping. This means that children would intend to help out-group peers compared with in-group peers more in the public setting when they perceived a positive descriptive norm about the out-group. Furthermore,

General discussion

The aim of the current research was to provide novel insight into children’s motives for intergroup helping. In two studies, we examined the unique contributions of children’s empathic tendency and peer group norms. Results show that children’s intention to help differed according to the level of need of the peers, whether helping was public or not, and the perceived descriptive norm about the out-group.

In both studies and for situations involving high need, more empathic children intended to

Acknowledgments

The authors thank the children, parents, and teachers for participating in the studies. Moreover, the authors are grateful to Aart Sierksma for his help in data collection.

References (78)

  • R. Banerjee et al.

    Upsetting others and provoking ridicule: Children’s reasoning about the self-presentational consequences of rule-violation

    British Journal of Developmental Psychology

    (2010)
  • R. Banerjee et al.

    Children’s explanations for self-presentational behavior

    European Journal of Social Psychology

    (1999)
  • R.F. Baumeister et al.

    The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation

    Psychological Bulletin

    (1995)
  • R.S. Bigler et al.

    Social categorization and the formation of intergroup attitudes in children

    Child Development

    (1997)
  • M.B. Brewer

    The social psychology of intergroup relations: Social categorization, ingroup bias, and outgroup prejudice

  • B.K. Bryant

    An index of empathy for children and adolescents

    Child Development

    (1982)
  • R.B. Cialdini et al.

    A focus theory of normative conduct: A theoretical refinement and reevaluation of the role of norms in human behavior

  • D.X. De França et al.

    Social norms and the expression of prejudice: The development of aversive racism in childhood

    European Journal of Social Psychology

    (2013)
  • M. Du Bois-Reymond

    Beelden van Nederlandse kinderen over Duitsland en de Duitsers

  • Y. Dunham et al.

    From categories to exemplars (and back again)

  • N. Eisenberg

    The caring child

    (1992)
  • N. Eisenberg et al.

    Prosocial development

  • N. Eisenberg et al.

    The development of empathic accuracy

  • N. Eisenberg et al.

    Prosocial development in middle childhood: A longitudinal study

    Developmental Psychology

    (1987)
  • N. Eisenberg et al.

    Brazilian adolescents’ prosocial moral judgment and behavior: Relations to sympathy, perspective taking, gender-role orientation, and demographic characteristics

    Child Development

    (2001)
  • E. Fehr et al.

    Egalitarianism in young children

    Nature

    (2008)
  • S. FitzRoy et al.

    Learning to control ethnic intergroup bias in childhood

    European Journal of Social Psychology

    (2010)
  • D.M. Gelfand et al.

    The effects of instructional prompts and praise on children’s donation dates

    Child Development

    (1975)
  • A. Geraci et al.

    The developmental roots of fairness: Infants’ reactions to equal and unequal distributions of resources

    Developmental Science

    (2011)
  • S.E. Hailey et al.

    A social psychologist’s guide to the development of racial bias: The critical role of status

    Social and Personality Psychology Compass

    (2013)
  • C.L. Hardy et al.

    Nice guys finish first: The competitive altruism hypothesis

    Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin

    (2006)
  • J.A. Hatch

    Impression management in kindergarten classrooms: An analysis of children’s face-work in peer interactions

    Anthropology and Education Quarterly

    (1987)
  • N. Hopkins et al.

    Helping to improve the group stereotype: On the strategic dimension of prosocial behavior

    Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin

    (2007)
  • R. Janoff-Bulman et al.

    Proscriptive versus prescriptive morality: Two faces of moral regulation

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (2009)
  • M. Killen et al.

    How children and adolescents evaluate gender and racial exclusion

    Monographs for the Society for Research in Child Development

    (2002)
  • M. Killen et al.

    Children and social exclusion: Morality, prejudice, and group identity

    (2011)
  • M. Killen et al.

    Development of intra- and intergroup judgments in the context of moral and social-conventional norms

    Child Development

    (2013)
  • M. Killen et al.

    Children’s social reasoning about inclusion and exclusion in gender and race peer group contexts

    Child Development

    (2001)
  • K. Kowalski et al.

    The influence of perceptual features, ethnic labels, and sociocultural information on the development of ethnic/racial bias in young children

    Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology

    (2001)
  • Cited by (56)

    • Empathy and group processes in Japanese preschool children: The odd one out among friends receives less empathic concern than out-groups

      2022, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology
      Citation Excerpt :

      In kindergartens, Japanese preschoolers are socialized to respond to a distressed peer with empathy (Hayashi et al., 2009). Although children have more empathy for in-group members, they help out-group members if there is a group norm for showing empathy for others regardless of group membership (Sierksma, Thijs, & Verkuyten, 2014). For Japanese children, being in a different peer group does not affect empathic experiences, unlike children in the Western context.

    • The consequences of outgroup helping: Children's and adolescents’ reasoning

      2021, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology
      Citation Excerpt :

      Children place more importance on intragroup helping (ingroup members being loyal to each other) over intergroup helping (Sierksma et al., 2014). In the case of intergroup helping, those who identify more with their own group are more likely to support intergroup helping when their group is benefiting from the helping (Sierksma et al., 2014). Thus, children integrate their feelings about their own group with an understanding of group interests when making decisions about outgroup helping.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text