Task- and age-dependent effects of visual stimulus properties on children’s explicit numerosity judgments

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2013.04.006Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Children’s numerosity judgments are affected by the visual stimulus manipulations.

  • The influence of the visual stimulus manipulations depends on the task at hand.

  • Children can be differentially influenced by the visual stimulus manipulations.

  • The effect of the visual stimulus manipulations changes throughout development.

  • The performance on the tasks is not associated with mathematics achievement.

Abstract

Researchers investigating numerosity processing manipulate the visual stimulus properties (e.g., surface). This is done to control for the confound between numerosity and its visual properties and should allow the examination of pure number processes. Nevertheless, several studies have shown that, despite different visual controls, visual cues remained to exert their influence on numerosity judgments. This study, therefore, investigated whether the impact of the visual stimulus manipulations on numerosity judgments is dependent on the task at hand (comparison task vs. same–different task) and whether this impact changes throughout development. In addition, we examined whether the influence of visual stimulus manipulations on numerosity judgments plays a role in the relation between performance on numerosity tasks and mathematics achievement. Our findings confirmed that the visual stimulus manipulations affect numerosity judgments; more important, we found that these influences changed with increasing age and differed between the comparison and the same–different tasks. Consequently, direct comparisons between numerosity studies using different tasks and age groups are difficult. No meaningful relationship between the performance on the comparison and same–different tasks and mathematics achievement was found in typically developing children, nor did we find consistent differences between children with and without mathematical learning disability (MLD).

Introduction

Numerosity and its continuous visual properties are correlated in everyday life. For instance, when more marbles are added to a collection of marbles, numerosity as well as the size of the collection increases. Therefore, it is common practice that studies investigating numerosity processing manipulate the visual cues of the numerosity stimuli. These manipulations should prevent participants from relying on the visual cues when judging numerosity and, thus, allow researchers to study pure number processes. However, an increasing number of studies show that numerosity judgments are sensitive to the continuous visual properties of the numerosity stimuli. These studies show, for instance, that numerosity judgments are influenced by density (Dakin et al., 2011, Sophian and Chu, 2008, Tibber et al., 2012), the size of the individual elements (Gebuis and Reynvoet, 2011b, Hurewitz et al., 2006, Rousselle and Noël, 2008, Tokita and Ishiguchi, 2010), or patch size (Gebuis and Gevers, 2011, Tokita and Ishiguchi, 2010).

Only a few researchers have investigated whether and how the visual stimulus manipulations they use in their experiments affect the measurements of numerosity processing. Researchers mostly believe that the visual confounds are sufficiently controlled and collapse the data from congruent trials (e.g., the larger numerosity has larger visual cues) and incongruent trials (e.g., the smaller numerosity has larger visual cues) without further examining whether the visual stimulus manipulations in these conditions lead to different findings (e.g., De Smedt and Gilmore, 2011, Piazza et al., 2004). However, investigating the effect of the different visual manipulations in congruent and incongruent trials separately seems to be necessary because some studies have shown that participants respond to the visual properties even when controlling for the visual cues (Gilmore et al., 2011, Inglis et al., 2011). For instance, approximately 30% of the 8-year-olds in a study by Inglis and colleagues (2011) and approximately 40% of the adults in a study by Gilmore and colleagues (2011) were removed. The performance of these participants in the congruent and incongruent conditions differed by more than 50% in accuracy, suggesting that they relied on the visual stimulus properties. Apparently, the performance on numerosity tasks is unavoidably affected by the manipulations of the continuous visual properties.

To allow comparisons across numerosity studies using different designs and including different age groups, it is essential to find out how the visual stimulus manipulations affect numerosity performance throughout development. It is not clear whether children of different ages are influenced differently by the visual stimulus properties. Infant studies have revealed inconsistent results about the effect of the visual stimulus properties on numerosity judgments. There are studies showing that infants might be more sensitive to the continuous visual properties at the expense of numerosity (Clearfield and Mix, 1999, Clearfield and Mix, 2001, Feigenson et al., 2002), that infants are equally sensitive to numerosity and visual cues (Brannon et al., 2006, Cordes and Brannon, 2009, vanMarle and Wynn, 2006), and that infants prefer to attend to numerosity over visual properties (Brannon et al., 2004, Cordes and Brannon, 2008, Cordes and Brannon, 2011, Xu, 2003, Xu and Spelke, 2000, Xu et al., 2005). The few studies using explicit paradigms, which examined the effects of the visual stimulus manipulations, showed a more consistent pattern of findings. These studies point to a reliance on visual cues in young children at the expense of number, which seems to decrease with increasing age (Rousselle and Noël, 2008, Rousselle et al., 2004) but remains visible even in adults (Gebuis and Van der Smagt, 2011, Gilmore et al., 2011, Halberda et al., 2008). However, the age at which children could compare numerosities while the visual cues were manipulated differed. Rousselle and colleagues (Rousselle and Noël, 2008, Rousselle et al., 2004) showed that 3-year-old children were unable to compare numerosities when the stimuli were controlled for surface area, whereas 4- and 5-year-olds performed significantly above chance. In contrast, Soltész, Szucs, and Szucs (2010) observed that 4-year-olds performed at chance level when the visual properties and numerosity were manipulated inconsistently, whereas performance was above chance from 5 years onward.

Research on the impact of the visual stimulus manipulations on numerosity judgments is necessary when considering the important conclusions that are drawn from the results of numerosity studies. It is, for instance, suggested that performance on numerosity tasks is related to mathematics achievement. Typically developing children who performed worse on numerosity discrimination showed less proficiency in mathematics than children who performed better on this task (e.g., Halberda et al., 2008, Inglis et al., 2011, Mazzocco et al., 2011b). Similarly, children with mathematical learning disability (MLD) performed worse than controls on numerosity comparison (Mazzocco et al., 2011a, Mussolin et al., 2010, Piazza et al., 2010). However, there are also several studies that show no correlation between the performance on numerosity tasks and mathematics achievement in typically developing children (e.g., Holloway and Ansari, 2009, Sasanguie et al., 2012, Soltész et al., 2010) or that show no difference between children with and without MLD on numerosity discrimination ability (De Smedt and Gilmore, 2011, Iuculano et al., 2008, Rousselle and Noël, 2007).

The idea has been proposed that the influence of the visual stimulus properties could explain the mixed findings regarding the relationship between numerosity processing and mathematics achievement (Gebuis & Reynvoet, in press). To date, only a few studies investigating the association between numerosity discrimination and mathematics achievement have taken the influence of the visual stimulus properties into account. Mussolin and colleagues (2010) suggested that children with MLD were more sensitive than controls to continuous visual properties, such as density and surface area, during comparison of random stick patterns. Similarly, Mejias, Mussolin, Rousselle, Grégoire, and Noël (2012) showed that, in contrast to control children, children with MLD relied more on the visual cues that correlated with numerosity because they made better estimations in a task with homogeneous-sized dots (where the cumulative area covaried with numerosity) than in a task with heterogeneous-sized dots (where the cumulative area was constant across numerosities). These studies suggest that children with MLD are more influenced by the visual stimulus properties than control participants.

Our goals in this study were twofold. First, we investigated whether the visual cues influence numerosity judgments differently across development and across task designs. Insight into these variables will reveal whether data from different studies can be compared or whether caution is warranted. Second, we investigated whether the visual cues could explain the observed discrepancies between numerosity studies regarding the relationship between numerosity processing and mathematics achievement. To this end, we conducted an experiment in a group of typically developing children from first, second, third, and sixth grades (Experiment 1) and in children with MLD (Experiment 2). The performance of both typically developing children and children with MLD was investigated because difficulties in typically developing children with low math proficiency are not necessarily the same as the difficulties in children with MLD (e.g., Desoete et al., 2012, Mazzocco et al., 2008). We administered both a comparison task and a same–different task in which we manipulated the visual properties of the numerosity stimuli using a more stringent method to control the visual cues than the method used in most previous studies (Gebuis & Reynvoet, 2011a). Similar to previous studies, in the comparison task, in half of the trials the numerically larger numerosity consisted of a larger surface, density, and diameter but a smaller convex hull (congruent condition), and in the other half the numerically larger numerosity consisted of a smaller surface, density, and diameter but a larger convex hull (incongruent condition) (for a similar design, see Condition 4 in Gebuis & Reynvoet, 2011a). The visual cues of the trials with two numerically different stimuli in the same–different task were manipulated in the same way as in the comparison task. The numerically same trials also consisted of a stimulus with larger visual cues and a stimulus with smaller visual cues. Differences in the reliance on the visual cues in a comparison task and a same–different task can be expected. Namely, in the comparison task, where participants need to indicate the numerically larger numerosity, participants might be prone to indicate the stimulus consisting of larger visual properties as numerically larger. In contrast, in the same–different task, where participants need to decide whether the numerosities are numerically the same or different, participants might be prone to judge the numerosities as different because the visual properties of the numerosities always differ (also in the numerically same trials, one of the two stimuli was always larger in visual stimulus properties). To examine the relationship between the performance on the tasks and mathematics achievement in Experiment 1, we administered both a general mathematics achievement test and a timed arithmetic test.

Section snippets

Participants

Participants were 26 first graders, 34 second graders, 34 third graders, and 30 sixth graders recruited from two elementary schools in Belgium. Among this sample, 6 participants were excluded from the analyses because they were too slow or made too many errors (>3 standard deviations above group average) or because their math achievement score was missing. This resulted in a final sample of 25 first graders (16 boys and 9 girls, mean age = 6.67 years, SD = 0.29), 32 second graders (20 boys and 12

Participants

The MLD group consisted of 33 primary school children with MLD. To be part of the MLD group, children were indicated by their teacher or therapist as having a severe delay in mathematics despite receiving intensive remediation (in and/or outside school). Among this sample, 12 children attended regular schools and 21 children attended special needs schools for children with learning disorders. Moreover, all children with MLD scored below the 10th percentile of the population sample mean on a

General discussion

In the current study, we examined how manipulations of the continuous visual properties affect children’s numerosity judgments. In Experiment 1, we investigated the development of the interaction between numerosity and its continuous visual properties in first, second, third, and sixth graders. Both a comparison task and a same–different task were administered as well as two standardized mathematics achievement tests. This enabled us to examine whether the effects of the visual stimulus

Acknowledgments

Emmy Defever and Bert Reynvoet were supported by the Fund for Scientific Research–Flanders (Grant G.0451.10) and the Research Fund K.U. Leuven, and Titia Gebuis was supported by the Marie Curie Intra European Fellowship (Grant 255140). The authors thank Steffi De Cnodder and Sam Van Snick for assistance with the data collection, Roi Cohen Kadosh for helpful discussion, and all participating children and their parents.

References (58)

  • L. Rousselle et al.

    Magnitude comparison in preschoolers: What counts? Influence of perceptual variables

    Journal of Experimental Child Psychology

    (2004)
  • D. Sasanguie et al.

    Approximate number sense, symbolic number processing, or number–space mappings: What underlies mathematics achievement?

    Journal of Experimental Child Psychology

    (2013)
  • C. Sophian et al.

    How do people apprehend large numerosities?

    Cognition

    (2008)
  • F. Xu

    Numerosity discrimination in infants: Evidence for two systems of representations

    Cognition

    (2003)
  • F. Xu et al.

    Large number discrimination in 6-month-old infants

    Cognition

    (2000)
  • E.M. Brannon et al.

    The development of area discrimination and its implications for number representation in infancy

    Developmental Science

    (2006)
  • Brus, B. T., & Voeten, M. J. M. (1999). Eén-Minuut-Test: Vorm A en B—Verantwoording en handleiding [One-Minute Reading...
  • J. Cantlon et al.

    Heterogeneity impairs numerical matching but not numerical ordering in preschool children

    Developmental Science

    (2007)
  • S. Censabella et al.

    The inhibition of exogenous distracting information in children with learning disabilities

    Journal of Learning Disabilities

    (2005)
  • M.W. Clearfield et al.

    Number versus contour length in infants’ discrimination of small visual sets

    Psychological Science

    (1999)
  • M.W. Clearfield et al.

    Infants use continuous quantity—not number—to discriminate small visual sets

    Journal of Cognition and Development

    (2001)
  • S. Cordes et al.

    The difficulties of representing continuous extent in infancy: Using number is just easier

    Child Development

    (2008)
  • S. Cordes et al.

    The relative salience of discrete and continuous quantity in young infants

    Developmental Science

    (2009)
  • S. Cordes et al.

    Attending to one of many: When infants are surprisingly poor at discriminating an item’s size

    Frontiers in Psychology

    (2011)
  • S.C. Dakin et al.

    A common visual metric for approximate number and density

    Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America

    (2011)
  • T. De Vos

    TTR: Tempotest rekenen [Tempo Test Arithmetic]

    (1992)
  • G. Deloof

    LVS–VCLB leerlingvolgsysteem: Wiskunde toetsen 6—basisboek [student monitoring system: Mathematics tests 6—manual]

    (2005)
  • A. Desoete et al.

    Can we predict mathematical learning disabilities from symbolic and non-symbolic comparison tasks in kindergarten? Findings from a longitudinal study

    British Journal of Educational Psychology

    (2012)
  • P. Dudal

    LVS–VCLB leerlingvolgsysteem: Wiskunde toetsen 1—Basisboek [student monitoring system: Mathematics tests 1—manual]

    (2000)
  • Cited by (34)

    • Evidence for the role of inhibition in numerical comparison: A negative priming study in 7- to 8-year-olds and adults

      2019, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology
      Citation Excerpt :

      Whereas most studies in preschoolers report congruency effects on accuracy rates (Fuhs & McNeil, 2013; Fuhs et al., 2016), studies in early primary school years report both less accurate and slower responses on incongruent trials, whereby dot area and/or total surface area of the dots were among the manipulated non-numerical dimensions (Gilmore et al., 2013; Szücs et al., 2013). Several studies have investigated the development of these congruency effects with age, showing that age influences the amplitude of the difference in performance between congruent and incongruent trials in nonsymbolic numerical comparison tasks (Defever et al., 2013; Gebuis, Herfs, Kenemans, de Haan, & van der Smagt, 2009; Soltész, Szücs, & Szücs, 2010; Szücs et al., 2013). In particular, studies have reported larger congruency effects between school-aged children and adults.

    • Spontaneous orientation towards irrelevant dimensions of magnitude and numerical acuity

      2018, Learning and Instruction
      Citation Excerpt :

      In infants, the relative sensitivity to number vs. non-numerical dimensions of magnitude has been commonly investigated by pitting these dimensions against each other using looking time paradigms (Cordes & Brannon, 2008, 2009, 2011; Brannon, Lutz, & Cordes, 2006; Clearfield & Mix, 2001; Feigenson, Carey, & Spelke, 2002; Libertus, Starr, & Brannon, 2014; Mix, Huttenlocher, & Levine, 2002; Starr & Brannon, 2015; Starr, Libertus, & Brannon, 2013). In preschoolers and school-aged children, several studies in past few years have investigated the impact of non-numerical dimensions of magnitude on numerical estimation by analyzing children's performance on congruent vs. incongruent trials (Defever, Reynvoet, & Gebuis, 2013; Fuhs & McNeil, 2013; Fuhs, McNeil, Kelley, O’Rear, & Villano, 2016; Gilmore, Cragg, Hogan, & Inglis, 2016; Szűcs, Nobes, Devine, Gabriel, & Gebuis, 2013). For instance, Szűcs et al. (2013) observed that these congruency effects were driving 7-year-old children's level of precision of their numerical estimation capacities, and that these congruency effects were larger than the ones observed in adults.

    • The contributions of numerical acuity and non-numerical stimulus features to the development of the number sense and symbolic math achievement

      2017, Cognition
      Citation Excerpt :

      The present work therefore both extends the use of the model and analysis framework developed by DeWind et al. (2015) to developmental populations and applies it to clarifying the relation between nonsymbolic numerical representations, symbolic math, and inhibitory control. Consistent with previous results (Allik & Tuulmets, 1991; Dakin et al., 2011; Defever et al., 2013; DeWind et al., 2015; Gebuis & Gevers, 2011; Gebuis & Reynvoet, 2011b, 2012a, 2012b; Ginsburg & Nicholls, 1988; Hurewitz et al., 2006; Rousselle & Noël, 2008; Rousselle et al., 2004; Soltész et al., 2010; Sophian & Chu, 2008; Szűcs et al., 2013; Tibber et al., 2012; Tokita & Ishiguchi, 2010, 2013), we found that numerical decision-making in all age groups was influenced by non-numerical features. When participants were attempting to make decisions based on the numerosity of the arrays, even adults were unable to ignore the spacing of items within the arrays.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text