Prosocial tendencies predict friendship quality, but not for popular children

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.04.002Get rights and content

Abstract

Is prosocial behavior a prerequisite for having good-quality friendships? This study (N = 477, mean age = 12.2 years) examined whether the link between children’s prosocial tendencies and their perceived friendship quality was dependent on children’s level of popularity in the peer group. Children’s prosocial tendencies were assessed both as observed behavior in a standardized setting and as a self-reported predisposition to act in prosocial ways. Across measures, the results showed that prosocial tendencies are associated with higher perceived friendship quality among nonpopular children (i.e., children holding average or lower levels of popularity), but not among popular children. Thus, even if they lack prosocial qualities, popular children are still able to hold good-quality friendships. Popular children may have other compensating characteristics, such as popularity by association, that make them attractive for peers to be friends with.

Highlights

► We examined the link between prosociality and friendship quality. ► Prosociality was assessed both as observed and self-reported behavior. ► Prosociality was related to friendship quality, but only among nonpopular children. ► Even if popular children are not prosocial, they can hold good quality friendships.

Introduction

Prosocial behavior—behavior intended to benefit other people—plays an important role in our social lives (Eisenberg, Fabes, & Spinrad, 2006). If humans were unable to show prosocial behaviors such as helping, sharing, and cooperating, it would be difficult to live in social groups, as humans do from the minute they are born. Nevertheless, children show pronounced individual differences in their tendencies to act in prosocial ways (e.g., Eisenberg et al., 1999, Eisenberg et al., 2006). These individual differences are likely to be consequential for the quality of their interpersonal relationships and their peer relationships in particular. Because prosocial behaviors benefit other people, it seems plausible to assume that these behaviors will typically be valued and rewarded by others (Asher & McDonald, 2009). Indeed, numerous studies have shown that prosocial children are better liked and accepted by their peers than less prosocial children (e.g., Asher and McDonald, 2009, Coie et al., 1990, Newcomb et al., 1993, Rubin et al., 2006).

Perhaps surprisingly, then, relatively little is known about how children’s prosocial tendencies relate to the quality of the friendships they hold. Friendships are a quintessential part of children’s social lives, satisfying basic needs of companionship, intimacy, and affection (Buhrmester, 1996, Furman and Collins, 2009). Friendships are dyadic, mutually rewarding bonds between children. They are defined by reciprocity: Peer bonds can be called friendships when both children gain benefits, or “social provisions” (e.g., intimacy, affection), from them (Bukowski, Newcomb, & Hartup, 1996). Such reciprocity is what distinguishes friendships from other peer bonds. The degree to which children obtain social provisions from a particular friendship is reflected in how they perceive the quality of that friendship (Furman & Buhrmester, 1992).

How may children’s prosocial tendencies be related to the quality of their friendships? According to the classical equity theory (Adams, 1965, Walster et al., 1976), people seek to maintain equity, or a just balance in provisions, in their relationships. In an equitable relationship, both partners receive commensurate benefits from the relationship relative to the contributions they invest. Because prosocial children have much to offer to their friends in terms of social provisions (e.g., instrumental aid, emotional support), they are likely to obtain many social provisions in return, and this should result in high levels of perceived friendship quality. Supporting that view, research shows that prosocial children’s friendships are of higher quality than those of less prosocial children (Cillessen et al., 2005, McDonald et al., 2011, Markiewicz et al., 2001).

Yet there is more for children to value in their friendships than their friends’ prosocial behavior alone. Children also attach great importance to being popular, especially during late childhood and adolescence (LaFontana & Cillessen, 2010), and so they typically prefer to associate with children high in popularity (with popularity defined as peer status based on prestige and visibility in the peer group, also referred to as perceived popularity; Cillessen, 2011). Popular children have many characteristics that make them attractive to be friends with. They tend to have fun and exciting social lives, they engage in many cross-gender interactions, they are socially powerful and visible, and they typically get much attention from teachers and classmates (Adler and Adler, 1998, Cillessen, 2011, Closson, 2009, Hawley et al., 2007, Vaillancourt and Hymel, 2006). Because peer status is contagious and mere association with a popular peer raises a child’s status (Dijkstra et al., 2010, Marks et al., 2011; cf. “basking in reflected glory effect,” Cialdini et al., 1976), it is not surprising that many children want to be friends with popular peers (Eder, 1985, Hawley et al., 2007). In fact, it may have so many benefits for children to befriend popular peers that they will require relatively little in return from such friendships in terms of reciprocated prosocial behavior. Thus, it is possible that popular children are able to attain good-quality friendships even when they show relatively low levels of prosocial behavior. Nonpopular children, by contrast, have less to offer in terms of “popularity by association,” and so it may be more important for them to show prosocial behavior to attain good-quality friendships—“you scratch my back and I’ll scratch yours.”

The few studies that were conducted in this area of research found positive correlations between popularity and friendship quality both in children and in adolescents (Litwack et al., 2010, Rose et al., 2004). To our knowledge, no previous work has examined whether children’s prosocial tendencies differentially impact their friendship quality, depending on whether they are popular or not. However, there is some preliminary evidence that the friendship quality of popular versus less popular children is dependent on different behavioral dispositions. One study found that children’s disposition to engage in relationally aggressive behaviors (e.g., gossiping, excluding others) may have a negative impact on the quality of their friendships, but only for nonpopular children, not for popular children (Rose et al., 2004). Similarly, we propose that the impact of children’s prosocial behavior on their friendship quality will depend on their level of popularity.

Indirect evidence for the notion that popular children should be able to attain good-quality friendships, even when showing relatively low levels of prosocial behavior, comes from Hawley’s research on adolescents’ resource control strategies. Popularity is positively related to the use of resource control strategies, which are strategies to get what you want in either a nice (“prosocial”) way or a not so nice (“coercive”) way. Hawley et al. (2007) found that both prosocial and bistrategic controllers experience high friendship quality compared with typical controllers. Thus, to the extent that bistrategic and prosocial controllers are usually popular in their peer group, Hawley’s work suggests that for popular children it is not necessary to be prosocial all of the time to hold high-quality friendships.

The aim of the current study was to examine to what extent the presumed link between children’s prosocial tendencies and their perceived best friendship quality is moderated by individual differences in popularity. We predicted that prosocial tendencies would be less strongly related to friendship quality for popular children because these children have multiple other resources and qualities that make them attractive to be friends with.

We measured prosocial tendencies using procedures from two fairly independent research traditions. Research in experimental social psychology has relied mainly on behavioral measures of prosocial behavior, typically obtained by observing helping behavior in controlled settings (Penner, Dovidio, Piliavin, & Schroeder, 2005). Research in personality psychology has relied mainly on self-report measures of one’s generalized tendency to act, feel, and think in prosocial ways, indexed by the Big Five trait of agreeableness (Penner et al., 2005). We used a controlled laboratory procedure to measure actual prosocial behavior. In a computerized task, participants ostensibly received e-mails from younger children who asked them for help, and the prosocial quality of participants’ responses was coded as a measure of prosocial behavior. We also administered a standard self-report measure of agreeableness.

Popularity and the related construct of likeability (i.e., how much children are liked by their classmates) were measured using peer ratings. The inclusion of likeability allowed us to assess the specificity of the predicted effects for popularity. Friendship quality was indexed by children’s perceptions of the social provisions they obtain from their friendships (Furman & Buhrmester, 1985). Because these provisions (e.g., companionship, affection, enhancement of worth) are subjective, we measured them using self-reports (Berndt and McCandless, 2009, Furman, 1996, Ladd, 2009).

Section snippets

Participants

Participants were 477 sixth graders (47% boys and 53% girls) whose ages ranged from 10 to 14 years (M = 12.2, SD = 0.5). They were recruited from 22 primary schools serving middle-class communities in The Netherlands. Most participants were of Dutch origin (81%); others were mainly of mixed cultural/ethnical origin. Informed parental consent was obtained for all participants (consent rates between schools ranged from 50 to 100%, with a mean consent rate of 80%). Among the sample, 10 children

Preliminary analyses

Table 1 shows the descriptive statistics and correlations between the study variables. Both helping behavior and agreeableness were significantly related to perceived friendship quality (ps < .01) but not to popularity (ps > .18). Helping behavior and agreeableness were not significantly correlated, as is often true for self-reported and behavioral indexes of conceptually related constructs.

Girls reported higher levels of friendship quality than boys, t(475) = −3.96, p < .01, d = 0.35. Girls also showed

Discussion

Do children hold higher quality friendships to the extent that they are more prosocial? It depends on their level of popularity. Specifically, the current study found that children’s prosocial tendencies (assessed either as observed behavior in a controlled setting or as a self-reported predisposition to act in prosocial ways) were associated with higher perceived friendship quality among children holding lower and average levels of popularity. For children holding higher levels of popularity,

Conclusion

Not all friendships are created equal. Although it is intuitive that children will hold closer and more affectionate peer relationships to the extent they are more prosocial, our findings indicate that there are exceptions. Popular children enjoy high-quality friendships regardless of whether they behave kindly toward others. In concert with findings from previous work (Cillessen et al., 2005, Litwack et al., 2010, Rose et al., 2004), it seems that for young adolescents there are at least two

References (47)

  • W.M. Bukowski et al.

    Friendship and its significance in childhood and adolescence. Introduction and comment

  • R.B. Cialdini et al.

    Basking in reflected glory: Three (football) field studies

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1976)
  • A.H.N. Cillessen

    Toward a theory of popularity

  • A.H.N. Cillessen et al.

    Predictors of dyadic friendship quality in adolescence

    International Journal of Behavioral Development

    (2005)
  • A.H.N. Cillessen et al.

    Conceptualizing and measuring popularity

  • L.A. Closson

    Aggressive and prosocial behaviors within early adolescent friendship cliques: What’s status got to do with it?

    Merrill–Palmer Quarterly: Journal of Developmental Psychology

    (2009)
  • G.L. Cohen et al.

    Peer contagion of aggression and health risk behavior among adolescent males: An experimental investigation of effects on public conduct and private attitudes

    Child Development

    (2006)
  • J.D. Coie et al.

    Peer group behavior and social status

  • J.J.A. Denissen et al.

    Development and validation of a Dutch translation of the Big Five Inventory (BFI)

    Journal of Personality Assessment

    (2008)
  • J.K. Dijkstra et al.

    Basking in reflected glory and its limits: Why adolescents hang out with popular peers

    Journal of Research on Adolescence

    (2010)
  • J.K. Dijkstra et al.

    The relation between popularity and aggressive, destructive, and norm-breaking behaviors: Moderating effects of athletic abilities, physical attractiveness, and prosociality

    Journal of Research on Adolescence

    (2009)
  • D. Eder

    The cycle of popularity: Interpersonal relations among female adolescents

    Sociology of Education

    (1985)
  • N. Eisenberg et al.

    Prosocial development

  • Cited by (22)

    • Link of outdoor exposure in daycare with attentional control and academic achievement in adolescence: Examining cognitive and social pathways

      2023, Journal of Environmental Psychology
      Citation Excerpt :

      Although the literature on the benefits of nature on prosocial behavior in children is limited, experimental research in adults has demonstrated that natural environments may enhance social connection among people (Goldy & Piff, 2020; Schertz et al., 2022), as well as stimulate intrinsic aspirations and generosity (Weinstein, Przybylski, & Ryan, 2009), which are important aspects of prosocial behavior and social bonding. In turn, both prosocial behavior (Brouwer & Engels, 2022; Collie, Martin, Roberts, & Nassar, 2018; Gerbino et al., 2019) and peer acceptance (Wentzel, 2017; Wentzel & Caldwell, 1997) have been found to predict better academic performance, possibly because they foster collaborative learning, positive social relationships, school motivation, and a sense of well-being (MacDonald, Côté, Eys, & Deakin, 2011; Poorthuis, Thomaes, Denissen, van Aken, & Orobio de Castro, 2012; Wentzel, 2017). Therefore, prosocial behavior and peer group acceptance could also serve as intermediate variables linking outdoor time in daycare to attentional control and academic achievement.

    • Preference and popularity as distinct forms of status: A meta-analytic review of 20 years of research

      2020, Journal of Adolescence
      Citation Excerpt :

      Across studies, the correlation between preference and popularity is typically moderate, as indicated by narrative reviews (see, e.g., Cillessen & Rose, 2005; Mayeux et al., 2011). At the same time, the details of individual studies have shown that the variation of the preference-popularity correlation has been large, ranging from as low as 0.20 in some studies (e.g., Bouman et al., 2012; Markovic & Bowker, 2015; Ojanen & Findley-Van Nostrand, 2014) to as high as 0.70 in other studies (e.g., Kuryluk, Cohen, & Audley-Piotrowski, 2011; Poorthuis, Thomaes, Denissen, van Aken, & de Castro, 2012; Zhang et al., 2018). The variability of this correlation is interesting.

    • What lies beneath peer acceptance in adolescence? Exploring the role of Nucleus Accumbens responsivity to self-serving and vicarious rewards

      2018, Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience
      Citation Excerpt :

      Potentially, the higher NAcc responses during receipt of self-serving rewards and the subsequent higher drive to obtain rewards for oneself might not be appreciated within the peer group. This is in line with prior findings that relate peer rejection to selfish behaviors (Carlson et al., 1984) and with results from research stressing the importance of other-regarding acts in successful peer relationships (Cillessen and Rose, 2005; Meuwese et al., 2016; Peters et al., 2010; Poorthuis et al., 2012). In the current study we did not find evidence for a relationship between NAcc responses during vicarious reward processing and peer acceptance.

    • Elective affinities matter as much as ethnicity in multi-ethnic schools

      2016, Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization
      Citation Excerpt :

      Existing empirical analyses of personality similarity among peers are limited to late adolescence. There is evidence both that boys and girls are more similar to their mutual best friend in three out of the five main personality traits − the Big Five − i.e. in Extraversion, Agreeableness and Mental Openness (Selfhout et al., 2007; Selfhout et al., 2010), and that personality matters for the quality of friendship (Poorthuis et al., 2012; Yu et al., 2014). Among the four individual characteristics we review (ethnicity, sex, intelligence and personality) the only joint impact analyzed is that of ethnicity and sex.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text