Methodological implications of the affect revolution: A 35-year review of emotion regulation assessment in children

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2011.03.009Get rights and content

Abstract

This investigation analyzed the methods used over the past 35 years to study emotion regulation (ER) in children. Articles published from 1975 through 2010 were identified in 42 child clinical, developmental, and emotion psychology journals. Overall, 61.1% of published ER articles relied on one method and 23.6% used two methods. Analyses revealed (a) 82.8% of published ER research occurring within the past decade; (b) higher rates of observational methods with infant and toddler/preschool samples, but more use of self-report methodology with middle childhood and adolescent samples; (c) a longer history of published ER research with samples of infants to 5-year-olds, including the use of more longitudinal design, compared with older samples; and (d) a positive association between journal impact ratings and the use of physiological and observational measurement. Review of the measurement tools used to capture ER revealed great diversity in how emotion processes are understood and evaluated.

Highlights

► A review of 157 articles revealed a dramatic increase over 35 years in the scientific inquiry of emotion regulation in youth. ► Designs using one assessment method were most common. ► The use of multi-modal emotion regulation assessment has not changed over time. ► A vast array of emotion regulation assessments have been developed to capture the diversity of systems involved in examining this construct.

Introduction

The regulation of emotion involves the management of diverse systems, including physiological arousal, facial and behavioral expressions, motivation, inter- and intrapersonal goals, and cognitive evaluations (Thompson, 1994). Due to this diversity in systems, it has been a challenge to find a common definition for emotion regulation (ER) and then develop and implement optimal assessment methods (Cole, Martin, & Dennis, 2004; Langlois, 2004). Measurement of ER is directly linked to theoretical issues regarding the definition and nature of the construct. Furthermore, debate has ensued regarding the conceptual and methodological dissociation of emotion from ER (see Child Development, Vol. 75, 2004), with some purporting a two-factor solution (emotion as preceding ER) (e.g., Cole et al., 2004) and others arguing for the inseparability of emotion from ER (e.g., Campos, Frankel, & Camras, 2004). As such, for this review, the following commonly accepted definition of ER is adopted: “the extrinsic and intrinsic processes responsible for monitoring, evaluating, and modifying emotional reactions, especially their intensive and temporal features, to accomplish one’s goals” (Thompson, 1994, pp. 27–28).

The study of ER in children is a relatively new field, with the past two decades described as an affect revolution (Fischer & Tangney, 1995) that has transformed how emotion processes have been conceptualized. However, the progress in assessment of these constructs lags behind and, as a result, limits the questions that can be addressed and makes the interpretation of mixed findings a daunting task. The overarching aim of this research was to provide documentation of how ER has been studied in children over the past 35 years because this information has the potential to provide insights concerning the strengths and limitations of the field and to offer guidance for future directions.

Even with a cleverly devised methodological system, the challenging nature of scientific inquiry into ER must be acknowledged. As Larsen and Prizmic-Larsen (2006) stated, “Emotion researchers need to keep clearly in mind that constructs are never purely measured. Rather, all measures are construct–method composites. … The theoretical meaning of a construct is given, in part, by the methods used to measure it” (p. 342). Because of the complexity of the constructs involved in ER and equifinality of dynamic systems, multilevel and multimethod approaches to the scientific inquiry of ER in both normative and atypical child samples have been advocated. Emotion is best defined as a multifaceted construct inferred from multiple indicators, and by focusing and isolating one aspect of emotion, an incomplete picture of the emotion system is likely to emerge (Larsen and Prizmic-Larsen, 2006). However, measurement in this system is complicated because the converging multiple response systems are loosely and imperfectly coupled, complexly interactive, and mutually influential. Thus, ER may be understood through accessing a variety of different processes from central and peripheral nervous system processes, sociocultural influences, and biological and behavioral contexts (Thompson, Lewis, & Calkins, 2008). The current study sought to systematically analyze the ways in which ER researchers have grappled with investigating this construct.

Researchers interested in studying children’s ER measure this construct primarily in four ways: self-report, other informant (parent, teacher, or peer), observational, and physiological–biological indicators (for reviews, see Morris, Robinson, & Eisenberg, 2006; Zeman, Klimes-Dougan, Cassano, & Adrian, 2007). Self-report methodologies provide an important assessment of ER even when the reporter may be a young child (Durbin, 2010). Individuals are the only ones able to assess and integrate a variety of levels of information about their own emotions (e.g., meld information about cognitions and physiological symptoms such as heart rate). However, self-report methods are limited by the demands they place on children such that they require children to be aware of emotions, have the ability to monitor emotions, recall their emotional experience retrospectively, and then communicate this information effectively to others (Zeman et al., 2007). The recent invention and use of electronic communication sources (e.g., electronic diaries) offers a partial solution to some of these issues, yet their incremental validity over other approaches is still questionable (Suveg, Payne, Thomassin, & Jacob, 2010).

Other reporters (e.g., parents, teachers) are also often enlisted to measure components of emotion because they provide an opportunity to understand children’s ER in diverse social settings. Although this information is considered to have better reliability and validity than self-reports (Morris et al., 2006), research has revealed that there are biases and other processes (e.g., parental psychopathology) that can affect ratings of children’s behavior (Fergusson, Lynskey, & Horwood, 1993). Although rarely explored, peer methods of nominations and emotion-based sociometrics could be a valuable method to explore the connection between ER and social processes (Perry-Parrish and Zeman, 2011).

Observational methods are often considered the “gold standard” in developmental research (Cummings, Davies, & Campbell, 2000). Body gestures, tones of voice, and facial expressions relay important information about emotional experiences, particularly emotional expression. In addition, observational coding systems can be developed in a flexible manner to capture the precipitants, reactions, and consequences of emotional expressions. However, an observational approach captures only some of the components of ER and does not tap into the associated internal unobservable processes. Furthermore, as children develop, so does their ability to dissemble emotional displays, thereby affecting the validity of this method (Saarni, 1984).

Neurophysiological systems are critically involved in the experience, production, and regulation of emotion (Curtis and Cicchetti, 2007). Recent evidence indicates that key neurocircuitry relevant to emotion processing undergoes several periods of reorganization well into adolescence (Giedd et al., 1999). Due to recent technological advances, a number of methods have evaluated different biological emotion systems. To investigate the activity of the cortico-limbic circuitry that is involved in emotion processing and regulation (LeDoux, 1996), electroencephalogram and neuroimaging techniques have been used to provide insight into these processes. The hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenocortical (HPA) axis is implicated in ER efforts, and studies with children have used cortisol assays to understand the impact of HPA disruption on ER efforts. Activation of the parasympathetic system, which is associated with ER abilities, has been measured by the vagal tone index (Porges, 1991).

Along with the increasing advances in the study of children’s ER, an acute awareness of the complexity of the construct has arisen; as such, achieving clarity in what constitutes ER and understanding the dynamic interplay between the developmentally important constituents has been slow to emerge (Thompson et al., 2008). As such, the overarching goal of this review was to evaluate the methodological state of the field of ER research using a multipronged approach. First, we sought to provide a description of the field with respect to validating the claim that we are indeed in the middle of an affect revolution. Second, we examined the number and type of measurement strategies used in the study of ER in children. Third, we investigated whether the use of certain methodologies to study ER differed as a function of developmental status. Fourth, we explored whether inquiries into typical versus atypical ER processes used different methodologies. Fifth, we investigated whether specific methodologies provided differential impacts on the understanding of ER in children. Sixth, we compiled a summary of the measures used to evaluate ER to provide researchers with a guide to the available instruments and methods as well as to illuminate gaps in our assessment toolbox. To meet these goals, 42 child clinical, developmental, and emotion psychology journals were reviewed to evaluate the methods used and their contributions to the study of childhood ER throughout a 35-year period.

Section snippets

Method

For a journal to be considered appropriate for review, it needed to be in the academic discipline of psychology and to have its primary focus on children (infants to 18-year-olds) or emotion processes; this yielded child clinical, developmental, and emotion journals. Articles on children’s ER published from 1976 through 2010 were selected from the following 42 journals (listed alphabetically with the number of articles located within each journal presented in parentheses): Advances in Child

Affect revolution

Overall, 157 studies were identified as being appropriate for the review. See the Appendix for a comprehensive listing of the publications, with some articles cited in the References section and denoted by asterisks. An examination of the number of studies published by decade revealed that the majority of studies that included ER as a variable were conducted between 2000 and 2010 (82.8%, n = 130), followed by 17.2% (n = 27) published from 1989 to 1999. There were no published studies with the key

Discussion

The findings of this 35-year review of the methods used to study children’s ER provide a valuable context from which to evaluate the state of the field and offer insight into avenues for growth. Quite striking is the evidence validating the claim made 16 years ago that we are indeed in the midst of an affect revolution (Fischer and Tangney, 1995). The rate of publications in children’s ER has increased dramatically since 1989, with 82.2% of the publications occurring within the past decade. If

References (150)

  • M.D. ∗Beijersbergen et al.

    Stress regulation in adolescents: Physiological reactivity during the adult attachment interview and conflict interaction

    Child Development

    (2008)
  • F. ∗Blanchard-Fields et al.

    The experience of sadness and anger in everyday problems impacts age differences in emotion regulation

    Developmental Psychology

    (2008)
  • J. Block et al.

    The California Child Q-Set

    (1980)
  • T.B. Brazelton

    The Neonatal Behavioral Assessment Scale

    (1973)
  • A.H. Buss et al.

    Temperament: Early developing personality traits

    (1984)
  • K.A. ∗Buss et al.

    Fear and anger regulation in infancy: Effects on the temporal dynamics of affective expression

    Child Development

    (1998)
  • S.D. ∗Calkins et al.

    Frustration in infancy: Implications for emotion regulation, physiological processes, and temperament

    Infancy

    (2002)
  • J.J. Campos et al.

    On the nature of emotion regulation

    Child Development

    (2004)
  • J. Cassidy et al.

    Family–peer connections: The roles of emotional expressiveness within the family and children’s understanding of emotions

    Child Development

    (1992)
  • J.F. Cohn et al.

    Three-month-old infants’ reaction to simulated maternal depression

    Child Development

    (1983)
  • P.M. ∗Cole et al.

    Preschoolers’ emotion regulation strategy understanding: Relations with emotion socialization and child self-regulation

    Social Development

    (2009)
  • P.M. Cole et al.

    Mood induction stimulus for children

    (1990)
  • P. Cole et al.

    Emotion regulation as a scientific construct: Challenges and directions for child development research

    Child development

    (2004)
  • P.M. ∗Cole et al.

    Mutual emotion regulation and the stability of conduct problems between preschool and early school age

    Development and Psychopathology

    (2003)
  • P.M. Cole et al.

    Individual differences in emotion regulation and behavior problems in preschool children

    Journal of Abnormal Psychology

    (1996)
  • P.M. ∗Cole et al.

    Expressive control during a disappointment: Variations related to preschoolers’ behavior problems

    Developmental Psychology

    (1994)
  • J.K. Connor-Smith et al.

    Response to stress in adolescence. Measurement of coping and involuntary engagement stress response

    Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology

    (2000)
  • J.M. ∗Contreras et al.

    Emotion regulation as a mediator of associations between mother–child attachment and peer relationships in middle childhood

    Journal of Family Psychology

    (2000)
  • J.A. Crowell et al.

    Mothers’ internal models of relationships and children’s behavioral and developmental status: A study of the mother–child interaction

    Child Development

    (1988)
  • M. Cummings

    Coping with background anger in early childhood

    Child Development

    (1987)
  • E.M. Cummings et al.

    Developmental psychopathology and family process: Theory, research, and clinical implications

    (2000)
  • J. ∗Curtis et al.

    Emotion and resilience. A multilevel investigation of hemispheric electroencephalogram asymmetry and emotion regulation in maltreated and nonmaltreated children

    Development and Psychopathology

    (2007)
  • E.L. Davis et al.

    Metacognitive emotion regulation: Children’s awareness that changing thoughts and goals can alleviate negative emotions

    Emotion

    (2010)
  • B.O. ∗De Castro et al.

    Emotions in social information processing and their relations with reactive and proactive aggression in referred aggressive boys

    Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology

    (2005)
  • R. DelCarmen-Wiggins

    Introduction to the special section: Transformative research on emotion regulation and dysregulation

    Child Development Perspectives

    (2008)
  • S. ∗Denham et al.

    Preschoolers at play: Co-socialisers of emotional and social competence

    International Journal of Behavioral Development

    (2001)
  • S.A. ∗Denham et al.

    Preschool emotional competence. Pathway to social competence

    Child Development

    (2003)
  • T.A. ∗Dennis et al.

    The late positive potential: A neurophysiological marker for emotion regulation in children

    Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry

    (2009)
  • A. ∗Dhariwal et al.

    Adolescent peer relationships and emerging adult romantic styles: A longitudinal study of youth in an Italian community

    Journal of Adolescent Research

    (2009)
  • G. Downey et al.

    Rejection sensitivity and children’s interpersonal difficulties

    Child Development

    (1998)
  • C.E. Durbin

    Validity of young children’s self-reports of their emotion in response to structured laboratory tasks

    Emotion

    (2010)
  • N. Eisenberg et al.

    The relations of emotionality and regulation to preschooler’s social skills and sociometric status

    Child Development

    (1993)
  • N. Eisenberg et al.

    The relations of children’s dispositional prosocial behavior to emotionality, regulation, and social functioning

    Child Development

    (1996)
  • N. Eisenberg et al.

    Parents’ reactions to children’s negative emotions: Relations to children’s social competence and comforting behavior

    Child Development

    (1996)
  • N. Eisenberg et al.

    The role of emotionality and regulation in children’s social functioning: A longitudinal study

    Child Development

    (1995)
  • N. Eisenberg et al.

    Relations among positive parenting, children’s effortful control, and externalizing problems: A three-wave longitudinal study

    Child Development

    (2005)
  • Ellis, L., & Rothbart, M. (1999). Early adolescent temperament questionnaire-revised (EATQ-R)....
  • R. Fabes et al.

    Young children’s coping with interpersonal anger

    Child Development

    (1992)
  • R. ∗Fabes et al.

    The relations of children’s emotion regulation to their vicarious emotional responses and comforting behaviors

    Child Development

    (1994)
  • R. ∗Feldman et al.

    Parenting stress, infant emotion regulation, maternal sensitivity, and cognitive development of triplets: A model for parent and child influences in a unique ecology

    Child Development

    (2004)
  • Cited by (161)

    • Editorial: Adding Measures of Emotion Dysregulation to Our Toolkits

      2023, Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry
    • Systematic Review: Questionnaire-Based Measurement of Emotion Dysregulation in Children and Adolescents

      2023, Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry
      Citation Excerpt :

      First, more than 100 questionnaires have been used to probe emotion (dys)regulation in youth since 2000. This is more than 3 times the number of self-, parent-, and other informant–reported measures identified by Adrian et al.27—an increase that illustrates the mission of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry 2020 Presidential Initiative.6 Second, closer examination of the questionnaires and their subscales reveals that measures assess disparate constructs and processes, whereby some capture the external manifestations of emotion dysregulation, while others capture its internal processes.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text