Brief Report
Physiology and functioning: Parents’ vagal tone, emotion socialization, and children’s emotion knowledge

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2008.03.007Get rights and content

Abstract

This study examined relationships among parents’ physiological regulation, their emotion socialization behaviors, and their children’s emotion knowledge. Parents’ resting cardiac vagal tone was measured, and parents provided information regarding their socialization behaviors and family emotional expressiveness. Their 4- or 5-year-old children (N = 42) participated in a laboratory session in which their knowledge of emotional facial expressions and situations was tested and their own resting vagal tone was monitored. Results showed that parents’ vagal tone was related to their socialization behaviors, and several parent socialization variables were related to their children’s emotion knowledge. These findings suggest that parents’ physiological regulation may affect the emotional development of their children by influencing their parenting behaviors.

Introduction

By preschool, children have arrived at the point where understanding of emotions becomes paramount. At this age, children begin to spend less time in their home environment and move to a setting in which their first nonfamilial social relationships are formed. They must rely less on the emotional coaching of their caregivers and begin to demonstrate affective competence in their independent social interactions. Emotion knowledge, or understanding the emotional reactions of others and the situations that likely cause them, is a key component of emotional competence and contributes to the development of the ability to engage in successful social interactions (Denham et al., 2003, Halberstadt et al., 2001). Children who are able to accurately read the emotional expressions of others and predict a likely emotional reaction in a given social situation can use this information to negotiate interpersonal interaction and successfully function in their new world of peer relationships. The current study employs a multimethod approach to investigate emotion situation knowledge and emotion expression knowledge in young children and probes the socialization and physiological concomitants of its development.

Parents must actively socialize their children to understand the norms of emotional behavior, a process often referred to as emotional socialization (Halberstadt et al., 1993, Pollak and Thoits, 1989). Parents socialize their children through means such as emotional discourse, reaction to their children’s emotions, and modeling. They can set the stage for their children’s future emotion knowledge both directly through teaching of emotional information and indirectly through their own emotional actions and reactions.

Previous research findings are consistent with the premise that children’s emotional competence develops, at least in part, within the context of parental emotion socialization. Studies have shown that parents who socialize their children by modeling positive expressive styles, showing high levels of emotional responsiveness, encouraging their children’s own emotional expression, and discussing emotional events have more affectively competent children (Denham et al., 1997, Denham et al., 1994). Relating directly to emotion knowledge, studies have also shown that children better understand emotion when parents are emotionally expressive and responsive (Denham & Grout, 1992) and foster an atmosphere of positive emotional expressiveness within the home (Denham & Kochanoff, 2002). Previous studies also indicate that adequate balance of both positive and negative emotions in the family environment is indicative of children’s adept emotional functioning (Eisenberg and Fabes, 1994, Valiente et al., 2006). Although the previous literature has established a link between parental socialization of emotion and children’s emotional competence, the specific mechanisms underlying this relationship are still under investigation.

Emotional behaviors, whether in adults or in children, are believed to have an underlying physiological basis and to be influenced by people’s physiological regulation. Cardiac vagal tone (VT), a measure of the variability in heart rate (Porges, 1995), is commonly used as an index of such regulation. Optimal resting VT is thought to underlie adaptive behavioral and emotional responses to environmental demands (Porges, Dousard-Roosevelt, & Maiti, 1994). Multiple studies of emotion during early childhood have found that high baseline VT is associated with several aspects of emotional competence, including but not limited to appropriate affective responding (Calkins, 1997), better emotion regulation (Fox, 1989), and fewer internalizing and externalizing behavior problems (El-Sheikh, Harger, & Whitson, 2001). However, it must be noted that a consistent relationship between VT and emotional competence is not always found (e.g., Eisenberg et al., 1995).

Although links between underlying physiological systems and overt emotional behaviors may develop during infancy and early childhood, they also are evident during adulthood (Butler et al., 2006, Gross, 2002). Thus, parents’ emotion socialization behaviors might be expected to be influenced by their own physiological regulation. For example, parents with higher resting VT might be more likely to foster a positive emotional environment and encourage emotional expression by their children. As described above, these socialization behaviors have been associated with children’s emotion competence and, in particular, the development of emotion knowledge. Furthermore, due to shared genes and environment, parents and children might possibly show similarity in the physiological systems that influence competent emotional functioning.

Thus, our study seeks to investigate the links between parental physiological regulation and their emotion socialization behaviors that, in turn, may affect children’s developing emotion knowledge. Although previous research has examined relationships between children’s physiological systems and overt emotional behavior, no study has addressed linkages among parents’ VT, their emotion socialization behaviors, and the emotional development of their children.

Section snippets

Participants

Participants were 44 4- or 5-year-olds (25 boys and 19 girls, mean age = 59.05 months, SD = 5.36) and their primary caregivers recruited from the local community. Most children were tested with their mothers; however, 3 children were tested with their fathers and 1 was tested with a grandmother because these were their primary caregivers. The ethnic distribution of the sample reflected that of the mid-sized, southeastern U.S. town (68% Caucasian, 23% African American, 9% Hispanic/Asian/Middle

Data reduction

To form more comprehensive measures of parental emotion socialization, subscales from the CCNES and SEFQ were combined. Following the theoretical distinction and empirical findings of Eisenberg and Fabes (1994), we created aggregate CCNES scores of dismissing behaviors and coaching behaviors. This was accomplished by totaling those subscales hypothesized to indicate negative parental emotion socialization practices (i.e., the minimizing, punitive, and distress reaction subscales) and those

Discussion

The current study is the first to demonstrate links between parents’ physiological regulation and their emotion socialization practices. Parents with higher resting VT reported more desirable emotion socialization behaviors. We also found that parents with high resting VT, indicative of better regulation abilities, had children who were more knowledgeable about emotional situations. In addition, replicating previous studies, we found relationships between parental emotion socialization and

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by a Duke Interdisciplinary Initiative in Social Psychology (DIISP) grant provided through Duke University. We gratefully acknowledge the participation of the children and families who volunteered to be part of our study. We also thank Elizabeth Kauffman and the Duke psychology and neuroscience graduate students for their help in creating project stimuli, Susanne Denham for allowing us to use her emotion knowledge task, and Parul Kakar, Jenny Feldman, Blair Burke,

References (32)

  • S.W. Porges

    Cardiac vagal tone: A physiological index of stress

    Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews

    (1995)
  • M.H. Bornstein et al.

    Child and mother cardiac vagal tone: Continuity, stability, and concordance across the first 5 years

    Developmental Psychology

    (2000)
  • E.A. Butler et al.

    Respiratory sinus arrhythmia, emotion, and emotion regulation during social interaction

    Psychophysiology

    (2006)
  • S.D. Calkins

    Cardiac vagal tone indices of temperamental reactivity and behavioral regulation in young children

    Developmental Psychobiology

    (1997)
  • L. Camras et al.

    Children’s understanding of emotional facial expressions and verbal labels

    Journal of Nonverbal Behavior

    (1985)
  • S.A. Denham

    Social cognition, prosocial behavior, and emotion in preschoolers: Contextual validation

    Child Development

    (1986)
  • S.A. Denham et al.

    Preschool emotional competence: Pathway to social competence?

    Child Development

    (2003)
  • S.A. Denham et al.

    Socialization of emotion: Pathway to preschoolers’ emotional and social competence

    Journal of Nonverbal Behavior

    (1992)
  • S.A. Denham et al.

    Parental contributions to preschoolers’ understanding of emotion

    Marriage & Family Review

    (2002)
  • S.A. Denham et al.

    Parental contributions to preschoolers’ emotional competence: Direct and indirect effects

    Motivation and Emotion

    (1997)
  • S.A. Denham et al.

    Socialization of preschoolers’ emotion understanding

    Developmental Psychology

    (1994)
  • N. Eisenberg et al.

    Mothers’ reactions to children’s negative emotions: Relations to children’s temperament and anger behavior

    Merrill–Palmer Quarterly

    (1994)
  • N. Eisenberg et al.

    The relations of maternal practices and characteristics to children’s vicarious emotional responsiveness

    Child Development

    (1992)
  • N. Eisenberg et al.

    The relations of children’s dispositional prosocial behavior to emotionality, regulation, and social functioning

    Child Development

    (1996)
  • N. Eisenberg et al.

    Parents’ reactions to children’s negative emotions: Relations to children’s social competence and comforting behavior

    Child Development

    (1996)
  • N. Eisenberg et al.

    The role of emotionality and regulation in children’s social functioning: A longitudinal study

    Child Development

    (1995)
  • Cited by (62)

    • A systematic review and meta-analysis of the association between parenting and child autonomic nervous system activity

      2022, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews
      Citation Excerpt :

      If a study presented both effect sizes that met inclusion criteria (e.g., cross-sectional or parenting measured before child ANS) and effect sizes that failed to meet inclusion criteria, the study was included but only the acceptable effect sizes were used in meta-analysis. Full text screening resulted in m = 153 eligible studies, of which m = 99 studies provided sufficient data for the calculation of an effect size (Abaied et al., 2018; Bell et al., 2018; Blandon et al., 2010; Borelli et al., 2017; Bosquet Enlow et al., 2014; Brown, 2007; Bubier et al., 2009; Burgess et al., 2003; Cai and Tu, 2020; Chen et al., 2015; Cho and Buss, 2017; Clark et al., 2016; Conradt and Ablow, 2010; Creaven et al., 2014; Creavey et al., 2018; Cui et al., 2019; Decarli et al., 2020; Diamond et al., 2012; Dixon-Gordon et al., 2020; Duprey et al., 2021; Eiden et al., 2018; Erath et al., 2009; Fagundes et al., 2012; Feldman, 2015; Feldman et al., 2010, 2013, 2014; Feldman and Eidelman, 2007; Fletcher et al., 2017; Fox et al., 2019; Gilissen et al., 2007, 2008; Giuliano et al., 2015, 2015; Grady and Callan, 2019; Graham et al., 2010; Graham et al., 2017; Hagan et al., 2016; Ham and Tronick, 2009; Han et al., 2020; Hastings et al., 2008a, 2008b, 2014a, 2014b, 2019a, 2019b; Hinnant et al., 2015, 2016; Holochwost et al., 2014, 2018; Huffman et al., 2020; Izard et al., 1991; James et al., 2017; Kaplan et al., 2008; Katz et al., 2020; Katz and Gottman, 1997; Kennedy et al., 2004; Kochanska et al., 2017; Li et al., 2019; McQuade et al., 2021; Miller et al., 2013; Miller-Slough and Dunsmore, 2020; Miskovic et al., 2009; Monti et al., 2014; Nelson et al., 2017; Oshri et al., 2020, 2021; Paret et al., 2015; Partington et al., 2018; Perlman et al., 2008; Perrone et al., 2016; Perry et al., 2012, 2013, 2014, 2018; Pollak et al., 2005; Porges et al., 2019; Quigley et al., 2017; Richardson et al., 2019; Rudd et al., 2017; Scrimgeour et al., 2016; Shakiba et al., 2020; Sharp et al., 2012; Sichko et al., 2018; Skibo et al., 2020; Skowron et al., 2014; Smiley et al., 2020; Stanger et al., 2018; Sturge-Apple et al., 2012; Sweet et al., 1999; Tabachnick et al., 2019, 2021; Taylor et al., 2013, 2015; Tu et al., 2014, 2017; Van der Graaff et al., 2016; Wagner et al., 2018; Welch et al., 2020; West et al., 2021; Willemen et al., 2008, 2009; Zeegers et al., 2018; Zhang et al., 2020). Requests for necessary data to compute effect sizes were sent to 71 study authors: 54 authors were contacted for all (or any) study effect sizes, in order to include the study; 21 authors were contacted for additional effect sizes, 18 of which were requests for recalculating effect sizes using task level scores into effect sizes using change scores, and 3 were requests for unreported effect sizes.

    • Testing TIK (Tuning in to Kids) with TEC (Test of Emotion Comprehension): Does enhanced emotion socialization improve child emotion understanding?

      2022, Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology
      Citation Excerpt :

      In a sample of 134 predominantly Caucasian, middle-income families of 3-, 4-, and 5-year-olds, they found that supportive parental ERSBs were related to better performance on the external components of emotion understanding in children. Perlman, Camras, and Pelphrey (2008) replicated these results using a computerized version of the puppet interview. In a study of 42 preschool children aged 4 to 5 years, they found evidence of a significant association between non-supportive reactions and the external components of emotion understanding.

    • Does the child brain rest?: An examination and interpretation of resting cognition in developmental cognitive neuroscience

      2020, NeuroImage
      Citation Excerpt :

      Finally, we suggest that resting state studies across ages consider a low cognitive engagement video stimulus in order to better approximate the constant information processing and behaving that we have discussed previously as typical of children. This has been suggested by other researchers in the context of neuroimaging (Greene et al., 2016; Vanderwal et al., 2015) and is also typical of physiological studies in young children requiring a resting baseline (Calkins, 1997; Calkins et al., 2001; Perlman et al., 2008). By shifting the standard resting stimulus in the field to a low stimulation video, we can obtain more consistent network measurements in adolescents and adults (Hasson et al., 2010) as well as improve comparisons across samples and therefore improve comparisons across age.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text