Elsevier

Journal of Criminal Justice

Volume 42, Issue 1, January–February 2014, Pages 10-25
Journal of Criminal Justice

Foundation for a temperament-based theory of antisocial behavior and criminal justice system involvement

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2013.11.001Get rights and content

Abstract

Background

Temperament has been shown to be associated with behavior for millennia but has not been explicitly used in a theory of crime.

Methods

This state-of-the-art review incorporates theory and research from over 300 studies from developmental psychology, psychiatry, genetics, neuroscience, and criminology to introduce a temperament-based theory of antisocial conduct with criminal justice system implications.

Findings

Two temperamental constructs—effortful control and negative emotionality—are significantly predictive of self-regulation deficits and behavioral problems in infancy, in toddlerhood, in childhood, in adolescence, and across adulthood.

Conclusion

Unlike other theories that focus merely on explaining problem behaviors, our temperament approach also explains negative and aversive interactions with criminal justice system practitioners and associated maladjustment or noncompliance with the criminal justice system. A program of research is also offered to examine and test the theory.

Introduction

Criminological theories come in three general varieties. One approach centers on individual-level characteristics that are believed to increase the liability for antisocial conduct. A second approach points to environmental conditions, often of a structural nature, that are believed to moderate behavior and increase the likelihood that an individual will become antisocial. The third approach points to social processes as the primary causal reason why persons end up engaging in antisocial behavior. All three approaches have merit and substantial empirical support, but most of these approaches lack qualities that would provide a unified understanding of antisocial conduct. To do so, we believe a theory must meet two essential requirements. First, the theory must be able to advance a construct that can explain antisocial conduct irrespective of setting or developmental stage. The theory must have predictive validity across the life course because its fundamental explanatory construct has omnibus power. Even though conduct problems in childhood, adolescence, and adulthood are not identical, they share an elemental nature (heterotypic continuity) that theory can capture and articulate. Second, and most criminological theories give short shrift to this point, the theory must be able to explain the criminal justice system consequences that are produced by the same phenomenon that drives the underlying antisocial conduct. The same thing that manifests in crime and related personal dysfunction also manifests in maladjustment to and noncompliance with the justice system.

In the social and behavioral sciences, the study of temperament yields the greatest potential for a unified theory of lifelong antisociality and subsequent criminal justice system behavior. The reason is that temperament, which we define as the stable, largely innate tendency with which an individual experiences the environment and regulates his or her responses to the environment, is a construct that instantiates the interaction of person and environmental influences on behavior.1 In other words, temperament is multifactorial and blends the person, structural/status, and interactional traditions of criminological theories described earlier.

Temperament is heritable, meaning that variance within a sample or population is partially attributable to genetic factors, relatively stable from birth throughout childhood, and apparent in adulthood when it is often referred to as personality (Bates, 1989, Goldsmith et al., 1987, Kagan, 1998, McCrae et al., 2000, Thomas and Chess, 1977). In this way, temperament constructs provide explanatory power whether examining conduct problems during toddlerhood, delinquency during adolescence, or recidivism while on correctional supervision during middle age (see Fig. 1).

A host of temperament constructs exist, and in our view, they are all important. But to truly understand and explain the essence of antisocial behavior across the life span, two interrelated constructs are essential: effortful control and negative emotionality.2 Drawing extensively on the temperament literature and cognate research in pediatrics, clinical psychology, developmental psychology, and the neurosciences, the current theoretical offering has four aims. First, we present the first criminological theory that explicitly and exclusively utilizes temperament as its central explanatory locus. Second, we increase the range of temperament beyond its traditional place in childhood to extend into middle and even late adulthood. Third—and we believe most importantly—we articulate a theory that explains the usual criminal justice system experiences of criminal offenders who have demonstrated life-long conduct problems. Thus, variance in effortful control and negative emotionality not only produces antisocial conduct throughout life, but also explains failure in interacting with criminal justice practitioners and completing correctional sentences. A focus on the salience of interpersonal negativity and the aversive reactions it produces comports with the negative moral connotations of antisocial behavior generally (DeLisi, 2005, Wilson, 1997, Wilson and Herrnstein, 1985). In this way, our theory meshes with the philosophical debates in the study of antisocial behavior that relate to the moral response to conduct problems. Fourth, a program of research is proffered toward testing the validity of the theory.

Section snippets

A brief history

The starting point for the study of temperament can be attributed to the work of Hippocrates (460-377 B.C.E.) and the four humours typology of four distinct temperaments. These were composed of the melancholic person was who described as moody and anxious with a predominance of black bile, the sanguine person was described as cheerful, spirited, and good natured with a predominance of blood, the choleric person who was angry and irritable with a yellow bile, and the phlegmatic person who is

Effortful control

Effortful control is defined as the ability to inhibit a dominant response in favor of performing a subdominant response (Rothbart, 1989). Along with attentional control (the ability or capacity to concentrate attention on a specific object or task), cognitive control (the use of executive functioning and intelligence to regulate conduct), and inhibitory control (the suppression of unwanted thoughts, emotions, and actions in order to shift focus according to the demands required by the

Negative emotionality

Negative emotionality, sometimes referred to as negative affectivity, encompasses a range of dimensions that taken together present a person who interacts with social actors and experiences the environment in a generally negative way (Clark, 2005). Temperament components of negative emotionality include frustration which relates to the interruption of ongoing tasks or the blocking of goals, fear which is the negative affect associated with the anticipation of distress, discomfort which is

Effortful control and negative emotionality dynamism

While it is useful to understand effortful control and negative emotionality as independent concepts, there is considerable evidence that the two variables are interrelated (Clark, 2005, Dir et al., 2013, Eisenberg, Spinrad and Eggum, 2010, Kochanska and Knaack, 2003, Moreland and Dumas, 2008, Stringaris and Goodman, 2009a, Stringaris and Goodman, 2009b) and work in tandem to increase antisocial behavior. As Nigg, Goldsmith, and Sachek (2004, p. 45) noted, “Unsurprisingly, early negative

Family Environments

The clearest evidence of the person-environment interaction in the development of antisocial conduct is seen in the family-crime literature in criminology and the attachment literature in developmental psychopathology. A robust literature has documented the ways that negative parenting practices contribute to antisocial development along with the ways that child temperamental traits interact with parenting practices to produce maladaptive outcomes (e.g., Barrett and Fleming, 2011, Bowlby, 1969,

Agenda for research

A temperament-based theory is the centerpiece of a rich research agenda situated between biological liability, contextual factors, proximal problem behavior outcomes, and distal criminal justice system involvement (see Fig. 2). As such, a variety of designs and methodologies over the life-course can be utilized. An advantage of bringing to bear a long tradition of research on temperament is that well-developed instruments exist to measure the quantitative traits of effortful control and

Discussion

There is obviously not a shortage of theoretical explanations for problem behaviors, externalizing symptoms, delinquency, and crime with entire literatures focused on conduct problems at various points in the life course, such as toddlerhood, childhood, adolescence, and adulthood. Usually, theory limits itself to explaining the underlying behavioral problems with the subsequent criminal justice implications left for other researchers, academicians, and practitioners. This is one reason why

References (311)

  • A.C. Belden et al.

    Temper tantrums in healthy versus depressed and disruptive preschoolers: defining tantrum behaviors associated with clinical problems

    The Journal of Pediatrics

    (2008)
  • M.L. Benson et al.

    Reintegration or stigmatization? Offenders’ expectations of community re-entry

    Journal of Criminal Justice

    (2011)
  • D. Boisvert et al.

    Genetic and environmental influences underlying the relationship between low self-control and substance use

    Journal of Criminal Justice

    (2013)
  • H.A. Chapman et al.

    Hearts and minds: Coordination of neurocognitive and cardiovascular regulation in children and adolescents

    Biological Psychology

    (2010)
  • E.A. Cipriano et al.

    Predicting preschool effortful control from toddler temperament and parenting behavior

    Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology

    (2010)
  • I.N. Defoe et al.

    Disentangling the relationship between delinquency and hyperactivity, low achievement, depression, and low socioeconomic status: Analysis of repeated longitudinal data

    Journal of Criminal Justice

    (2013)
  • G.A. DeGangi et al.

    Psychophysiological characteristics of the regulatory disordered infant

    Infant Behavior and Development

    (1991)
  • M. DeLisi

    Self-control theory: The Tyrannosaurus rex of criminology is poised to devour criminal justice

    Journal of Criminal Justice

    (2011)
  • M. DeLisi et al.

    Alias: Lying to the police and pathological criminal behavior

    Journal of Forensic and Legal Medicine

    (2013)
  • M. DeLisi et al.

    New frontiers in criminal careers research, 2000–2011: A state-of-the-art review

    Journal of Criminal Justice

    (2011)
  • R. Agnew

    A revised strain theory of delinquency

    Social Forces

    (1985)
  • R. Agnew

    Foundation for a general strain theory of crime and delinquency

    Criminology

    (1992)
  • R.S. Agnew

    Storylines as a neglected cause of crime

    Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency

    (2006)
  • R.S. Agnew

    Strain theory and violent behavior

  • G. Alessandri et al.

    On the cross‐cultural replicability of the resilient, undercontrolled, and overcontrolled personality types

    Journal of Personality

    (2013)
  • N. Arikha

    Passions and tempers: A history of the humours

    (2008)
  • W.F. Arsenio et al.

    Affective predictors of preschoolers' aggression and peer acceptance: direct and indirect effects

    Developmental Psychology

    (2000)
  • J.G. Auerbach et al.

    DRD4 related to infant attention and information processing: A developmental link to ADHD?

    Psychiatric Genetics

    (2001)
  • R.H. Baillargeon et al.

    Opposition‐defiance in the second year of life: A population‐based cohort study

    Infancy

    (2011)
  • J.C. Barnes et al.

    Analyzing the origins of childhood externalizing behavioral problems

    Developmental Psychology

    (2013)
  • J. Barrett et al.

    Annual research review: All mothers are not created equal: Neural and psychobiological perspectives on mothering and the importance of individual differences

    Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry

    (2011)
  • J.E. Bates

    Concepts and measures of temperament

  • R.F. Baumeister

    Self-control, fluctuating willpower, and forensic practice

    Journal of Forensic Practice

    (2013)
  • R.F. Baumeister et al.

    Self-control, morality, and human strength

    Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology

    (2000)
  • K.M. Beaver et al.

    Low self-control and contact with the criminal justice system in a nationally representative sample of males

    Justice Quarterly

    (2009)
  • K.M. Beaver et al.

    The intersection of genes and neuropsychological deficits in the prediction of adolescent delinquency and low self-control

    International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology

    (2010)
  • K.M. Beaver et al.

    The relationship between self‐control and language: evidence of a shared etiological pathway

    Criminology

    (2008)
  • K.M. Beaver et al.

    Gene-environment interplay and delinquent involvement: Evidence of direct, indirect, and interactive effects

    Journal of Adolescent Research

    (2009)
  • J. Belsky et al.

    Cumulative-genetic plasticity, parenting and adolescent self-regulation

    Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry

    (2011)
  • J. Belsky et al.

    Mothering, fathering, and infant negativity as antecedents of boys' externalizing problems and inhibition at age 3 years: Differential susceptibility to rearing experience?

    Development and Psychopathology

    (1998)
  • J. Belsky et al.

    Beyond diathesis stress: differential susceptibility to environmental influences

    Psychological Bulletin

    (2009)
  • J. Belsky et al.

    The nature (and nurture?) of plasticity in early human development

    Perspectives on Psychological Science

    (2009)
  • C. Blair

    School readiness: Integrating cognition and emotion in a neurobiological conceptualization of children's functioning at school entry

    American Psychologist

    (2002)
  • R.J.R. Blair

    The neurobiology of psychopathic traits in youths

    Nature Reviews Neuroscience

    (2013)
  • C. Blair et al.

    Biological processes in prevention and intervention: The promotion of self-regulation as a means of preventing school failure

    Development and Psychopathology

    (2008)
  • J. Blair et al.

    The psychopath: Emotion and the brain

    (2005)
  • C. Blair et al.

    Physiological and neuropsychological correlates of approach/withdrawal tendencies in preschool: further examination of the behavioral inhibition system/behavioral activation system scales for young children

    Developmental Psychobiology

    (2004)
  • C. Blair et al.

    Relating effortful control, executive function, and false belief understanding to emerging math and literacy ability in kindergarten

    Child Development

    (2007)
  • T. Bogg et al.

    Conscientiousness and health-related behaviors: A meta-analysis of the leading behavioral contributors to morality

    Psychological Bulletin

    (2004)
  • D. Boisvert et al.

    Genetic and environmental overlap between low self-control and delinquency

    Journal of Quantitative Criminology

    (2012)
  • Cited by (237)

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text