How can we enhance cognitive bias modification techniques? The effects of prospective cognition

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbtep.2015.03.007Get rights and content

Highlights

  • A lack of positive prospective cognition is associated with depressive mood.

  • Inducing prospective positive cognition would have beneficial effect on mood.

  • Enhanced CBM-errors aims to promote prospective positive cognition.

  • Investigate differential effects between enhanced and standard CBM-errors.

  • Enhanced CBM-errors reduced negative interpretation bias.

Abstract

Background and objectives

Cognitive bias modification for interpretation, a computerized program which manipulates biased interpretations, has shown therapeutic promise, including evidence that negatively biased interpretations can be reduced, leading to corresponding improvements in symptoms. Cognitive bias modification for cognitive errors (CBM-errors) is a second generation CBM-I procedure which manipulates seven types of cognitive error and is especially relevant to depressive cognition. The aim of this study was to investigate whether the effects of the CBM-errors manipulation would be enhanced by adding a component facilitating prospective cognition to help embed and extend newly acquired interpretations.

Methods

A sample of 80 volunteers completed a single session experiment. With a pretraining-posttraining design, we compared the effects of enhanced CBM-errors (targeting cognitive errors plus prospective cognition) with standard CBM-errors (targeting cognitive errors without prospective cognition), on interpretations of new material and mood.

Results

Significant differences between enhanced and standard CBM-errors revealed that enhanced positive training was more effective at decreasing negative interpretations compared to the standard procedure.

Limitations

Extending the current investigation to a sample dysphoria or depression is needed for an appropriate next step.

Conclusion

The findings serve as ‘a proof of principle’ for the potential of prospective cognition to enhance the effects of CBM-errors and other similar CBM procedures. Further work to enhance the effectiveness of CBM procedures is needed.

Introduction

Changing cognitive bias is considered a powerful therapeutic mechanism for mood disorders. Cognitive Bias Modification that targets interpretation biases (CBM-I) is a computerised experimental method for modifying the interpretation of emotionally ambiguous information, and has shown promising therapeutic effects in subclinical and clinical disorders (Blackwell and Holmes, 2010, Lang et al., 2012, Lang et al., 2009, Lester et al., 2011, Yiend et al., 2014, Yiend et al., 2014). The core concept of CBM-I is to train people to interpret emotionally ambiguous information in a consistent direction, and CBM-I usually aims to shift spontaneously negative interpretations towards more benign or positive alternatives (Mathews & Mackintosh, 2000). The additional evidence that CBM-I can elicit mood, symptom and stress response changes has piqued interest in the development of CBM-I as a potential therapeutic tool.

Original versions of CBM-I were devised by Mathews and colleagues (Grey and Mathews, 2000, Mathews and Mackintosh, 2000). For example, Grey and Mathews (2000) trained unselected participants to interpret an ambiguous homograph (e.g., stroke, batter) in either a threatening or a positive way, and found that practice in disambiguating these words resulted in similar interpretations being made when responding to previously unseen words. Similar results were found in a study using more naturalistic material in the form of short emotionally ambiguous text passages related to social anxiety (Mathews & Mackintosh, 2000). In this and subsequent studies using this method, researchers have found training-congruent interpretations are applied to new ambiguous materials, as well as effects on state anxiety, response to stress (e.g. Wilson et al., 2006) and symptom reduction (Amir & Taylor, 2012).

Cognitive Bias Modification targeting cognitive errors (CBM-errors), was developed from the previously described text method. CBM-errors differs from previous versions of CBM targeting interpretation, in two main ways. First, the approach differs conceptually in that it broadens the content and cognitive processes targeted by training to go beyond interpretation of emotional ambiguity which are used by earlier versions of CBM for interpretation (CBM-I). This is because CBM-errors was specifically designed to manipulate the cognitive error categories originally identified by Beck, known to be prevalent in depression, and targeted during cognitive therapy (Lester et al., 2011). Second, the method seeks to improve the face validity of CBM content to reflect the full range and type of cognitive errors typically targeted in therapy. Thus, items were not experimenter generated (as in previous CBM-I methods), but instead, were developed from specific exemplars generated in the clinic by clinicians practising CBT and their patients (see Lester et al. for full details). Thus CBM-errors includes content which targets the 7 categories of cognitive errors identified by Beck and colleagues (Beck, Rush, Shaw, & Emory, 1979); selective abstraction, minimization, magnification, black and white thinking, personalization, overgeneralization and arbitrary inference (please see Yiend et al., 2014, Yiend et al., 2014 for examples of modification items). A recent meta-analysis of CBM studies, however, indicated that the effect sizes of CBM are smaller than previously reported (Hallion & Ruscio, 2011). In a subsequent study using CBM-errors in clinically depressed groups Yiend et al., 2014, Yiend et al., 2014 found that a single session of CBM-error training increased positive interpretations but there was no significant change in mood or emotional reactivity to a stressor. As such, one of the key questions in CBM research generally, and for CBM-errors in particular, is what factors could enhance its efficacy? Investigating factors that enhance CBM effects could have significant therapeutic implications and is being called for throughout the field (Fox, Mackintosh, & Holmes, 2014).

We chose to focus on depression in the current study because CBM studies in depression are lacking although there is convincing evidence of a negative interpretive bias in depression (Lawson et al., 2002, Wisco and Nolen-Hoeksema, 2010). In addition the long term outcome of depression treated in primary care is worse than previously thought (Yiend, Merritt, Burns, Lester, & Paykel, 2009) suggesting that developing new, low cost, widely accessible and effective interventions for depression is an increasing priority for services.

Several previous studies have already attempted to examine factors which might contribute to the efficacy of the CBM procedure. For example, the transfer effect of CBM training was larger in conditions requiring active generation and selection of emotional meanings, compared to passive exposure during CBM-I training (Hoppitt, Mathews, Yiend, & Mackintosh, 2010). Others have demonstrated that using imagery rather than semantic-verbal processing during CBM-I training is more effective in producing emotional change (Holmes, Lang, & Shah, 2009). However, none of these studies has examined the role of prospective (i.e. directed towards the future rather than the present) cognition in the effects of CBM-I.

Positive prospective cognition includes a tendency to expect favourable and hopeful life outcomes, and this is characteristic of healthy people who show optimistic biases about the future (Weinstein & Klein, 1996). In contrast, depressed and dysphoric people show a lack of positive expectations about the future (MacLeod & Salaminiou, 2001), and this is associated with suicidal ideations (Holmes et al., 2007, Williams et al., 1996). MacLeod and Byrne (1996) examined the number of positive and negative future events generated by people with anxiety only, anxiety and depression and healthy controls. Only those with depression, as well as anxiety, showed reduced anticipation of future positive experiences, whereas as both groups showed greater expectation of future negative experiences compared to controls. Similarly, MacLeod, Tata, Kentish, and Jacobsen (1997) found that a lack of positive future cognitions, but not an excess of negative future cognitions was observed in depressed patients. Miranda et al., 2008, Miranda and Mennin, 2007) provided supporting evidence that a lack of positive future expectations was associated with depression but not generalized anxiety disorder. Holmes, Lang, Moulds, and Steel (2008) also found that a highly dysphoric group appeared to show less vivid positive prospective imagery than a low dysphoria group. In line with previous evidence regarding the relationship between prospective cognition and emotional well-being, research proposes that cognition with a future time perspective might influence cognitive bias information processing (see Demeyer & De Raedt, 2014 for a review). That is, future time perspective cognitions, related to specific goals and motivational preferences lead to a preference toward positive information and away from negative information (Carstensen & Mikels, 2005). Demeyer and De Raedt (2014) found limited evidence that a more expansive future time perspective was related to avoidance of negative information. Similarly, the Reconstructive Memory Model (RMM) and Valuation Model (VM) explain that repetitive practice of positive future-oriented scenarios (RMM) might promote attribution of lower risk estimates and allocation of processing resources to positive-oriented stimuli, which increases expectations for occurrence of the events one practice (VM) (Miloyan, Pachana, & Suddendorf, 2014). Based on previous findings, it seems that prospective cognition/imagery might play a key role in emotional well-being and information processing.

The research reviewed above suggests that a deficit in positive prospective cognition might be specifically associated with depression and cognitive bias. When we think about the future, we mentally project ourselves forward to events using imagery (Suddendorf & Corballis, 2007). This suggests that adapting CBM techniques to include the induction of positive prospective cognitions with imagery could be one way of improving the efficacy of CBM for depression. In the present study we set out to investigate this by comparing an adapted version of CBM-errors, designed to promote prospective cognitions using imagery, with the previously reported standard CBM-errors procedure, that focuses on the modification of ‘present moment’ cognitive errors alone. Any differential effect between standard CBM-errors and enhance CBM-errors would therefore be attributable to the added component of generating prospective cognition. Before applying prospective cognition component to clinically depressed population, as a first step, we included a non-clinical population. Any beneficial findings regarding prospective cognition may serve as a proof of principle evidence for future clinical adaptation of CBM paradigm using prospective cognition, especially for depressed population. Our design contrasted positive and negative training directions in a non clinical sample, therefore including four training groups in total: standard positive (SP), standard negative (SN), enhanced positive (EP), and enhanced negative (EN). Based on previous findings on the role of prospective cognition on mood or cognitive bias, hypothesis testing examined whether the effects of enhanced versus standard training differed, when analysing positive training and negative training separately. We predicted that enhanced, compared to standard, trained groups would show greater changes of state mood (happy, sad) and interpretive bias (positive, negative) over time (pretraining vs. postraining) in a direction consistent with either the positive or negative training condition. Specific hypotheses were as follows:

Hypothesis 1

mood measures: There would be a significantly larger increase in happiness and decrease in sadness from pre-training to post-training in the EP group compared to the SP group (and vice versa for the EN compared to SN groups).

Hypothesis 2

bias measures: participants in the EP group would show an increase over time (baseline to test) in positive bias and decrease in negative bias, as measured by the Similarity Rating Test (SRT). Conversely, participants in the EN group would report higher negative bias and lower positive bias on the SRT than those in the SN group at test compared to baseline

Section snippets

Participants

Participants were recruited at King's College London via poster and circular emails. Participants were informed that the aim of the present study was to investigate how future directed cognition was related to depressive relevant thought and mood. Inclusion criteria were English fluency and no history of psychiatric disorders, psychotherapy treatment or psychiatric medication and score within the range of minimum 0 and maximum 13 on the BDI-II. Two participants were excluded after data

Participant characteristics

Randomisation to group resulted in the following number of participants in each: SP = 20, SN = 20, EP = 20, EN = 20. To check that no differences existed between groups prior to the experiment, an analysis of variance was conducted using Group (SP, SN, EP, EN) as the between subjects variable and participant characteristics as dependent variables. All four groups were comparable at baseline on all measures: age, F(3, 78) = 1.02, p = 0.39; trait positive affect, F(3, 78) = 1.00, p = 0.40, and

Discussion

Recent studies have drawn attention to the need for further investigation of ways to enhance the efficacy of CBM, if future clinical application is to be effective. As depression is characterized by a lack of prospective positive cognition, this is one obvious candidate mechanism which may improve CBM techniques aimed at depression. In the present study we therefore investigated whether including a component of prospective cognition with imagery would add to the efficacy of one particular CBM

Declaration of interest

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Funding organizations

The author(s) received no financial support for the research and/or authorship of this article.

Acknowledgements

We thank Helen Aivazian and Faridah Newman for their assistance on revising the materials used in the CBM procedures.

References (45)

  • B.E. Wisco et al.

    Interpretation bias and depressive symptoms: the role of self-relevance

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (2010)
  • J. Yiend et al.

    Enduring consequences of experimentally induced biases in interpretation

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (2005)
  • J. Yiend et al.

    Long term outcome of primary care depression

    Journal of Affective Disorders

    (2009)
  • N. Amir et al.

    Interpretation training in individuals with generalized social anxiety disorder: a randomized controlled trial

    Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology

    (2012)
  • A.T. Beck et al.

    Cognitive therapy for depression

    (1979)
  • A.T. Beck et al.

    Beck depression inventory (BDI-II)

    (1996)
  • S.E. Blackwell et al.

    Modifying interpretation and imagination in clinical depression: a single case series using cognitive bias modification

    Applied Cognitive Psychology

    (2010)
  • L.L. Carstensen et al.

    At the intersection of emotion and cognition – aging and the positivity effect

    Current Directions in Psychological Science

    (2005)
  • D.F. Cella et al.

    Reliability and concurrent validity of three visual-analogue mood scales

    Psychological Reports

    (1986)
  • I. Demeyer et al.

    The effect of future time perspective manipulation on affect and attentional bias

    Cognitive Therapy and Research

    (2014)
  • F. Faul et al.

    G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences

    Behavior Research Methods

    (2007)
  • E. Fox et al.

    Travellers' tales in cognitive bias modification research: a commentary on the special issue

    Cognitive Therapy and Research

    (2014)
  • Cited by (17)

    • Perfectly imperfect: The use of cognitive bias modification to reduce perfectionism

      2019, Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry
      Citation Excerpt :

      Future research is also needed to test if higher doses of CBM-I for perfectionism can lead to reductions in relevant psychopathology through changes in perfectionistic thinking. Additionally, further development and testing of this intervention is required in accordance with recent improvements in CBM-I methodology to improve participant engagement and intervention effectiveness (for example including an imagery component and asking participants to complete prospective cognitions about what happens next in the scenario; Lee et al., 2015; Menne-Lothmann et al., 2014). Given that the present intervention is similar to an intervention previously developed by Yiend et al. (2011), future work to develop CBM-I for maladaptive perfectionism should also compare these two alternatives in an effort to integrate them into a single—and ideally more robust—intervention.

    • Negative Interpretation Biases Precede the Onset of Psychosis

      2019, Behavior Therapy
      Citation Excerpt :

      So called ‘cognitive bias modification’ techniques are experimental procedures which have been designed to induce biases in an individual participant (for example create a positive or negative bias in those participating in the procedure); thereafter changes in symptoms or proxy symptoms can be measured. Over a decade of work has established the feasibility and safety of these procedures in both healthy (e.g., Mathews et al., 2007; Lee et al., 2015) and clinical samples (Yiend, Parnes, Shepherd, Roche, and Cooper, 2014; Yiend, Lee, Tekes, Atkins, Mathews, Vrinten, et al., 2014; Cardi et al., 2015). Future work on biased interpretation in psychosis could seek to test the causal hypothesis by applying an interpretation bias modification procedure specifically designed to target biases relevant to paranoia in healthy, vulnerable or even clinical groups.

    • Persecutory delusions: effects of Cognitive Bias Modification for Interpretation and the Maudsley Review Training Programme on social anxiety, jumping to conclusions, belief inflexibility and paranoia

      2018, Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry
      Citation Excerpt :

      Augmentation of CBM-I using computerised self-immersion or behavioural experiments may prove to be more effective for persecutory delusions. Recent studies have indicated that CBM-I tasks enhanced with prospective cognition to help consolidation of newly acquired interpretations may significantly boost its effectiveness (e.g., Lee, Matthews, Shergill, Yiu Chan, Majeed, & Yiend, 2015), although this will require replication in social anxiety and persecutory ideation. In summary, this study yielded limited support for an effect of CBM-I on social anxiety in 3/12 cases and for an effect of CBM-I on paranoia in 6/12 cases.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    1

    JongSun Lee is now at the department of psychology, Korea University, Republic of Korea.

    View full text