A randomized controlled trial of attention modification for social anxiety disorder

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2015.03.011Get rights and content

Highlights

  • An RCT of a modified attention bias protocol using words to reducing social anxiety.

  • Compares in-lab and at home administrations of the protocol.

  • Active and control conditions resulted in reductions in self-reported social anxiety.

  • No differences were found based on administration location or treatment condition.

Abstract

Social Anxiety Disorder (SAD) models implicate social threat cue vigilance (i.e., attentional biases) in symptom development and maintenance. A modified dot-probe protocol has been shown to reduce SAD symptoms, in some but not all studies, presumably by modifying an attentional bias. The current randomized controlled trial was designed to replicate and extend such research. Participants included treatment-seeking adults (n = 108; 58% women) who met diagnostic criteria for SAD. Participants were randomly assigned to a standard (i.e., control) or modified (i.e., active) dot-probe protocol condition and to participate in-lab or at home. The protocol involved twice-weekly 15-min sessions, for 4 weeks, with questionnaires completed at baseline, post-treatment, 4-month follow-up, and 8-month follow-up. Symptom reports were assessed with repeated measures mixed hierarchical modeling. There was a main effect of time from baseline to post-treatment wherein social anxiety symptoms declined significantly (p < .05) but depression and trait anxiety did not (p > .05). There were no significant interactions based on condition or participation location (ps > .05). Reductions were maintained at 8-month follow-up. Symptom reductions were not correlated with threat biases as indexed by the dot-probe task. The modified and standard protocol both produced significant sustained symptom reductions, whether administered in-lab or at home. There were no robust differences based on protocol type. As such, the mechanisms for benefits associated with modified dot-probe protocols warrant additional research.

Introduction

Social Anxiety Disorder (SAD; American Psychiatric Association, 2013) has significant lifetime (12.1%) and 12-month (7.1%) prevalence rates (Ruscio et al., 2008), and comparable rates for men and women (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). The disorder is marked by nervousness or discomfort in social situations (Antony & Swinson, 2000), resulting from fears of being evaluated, embarrassed (Weeks et al., 2010a, Weeks et al., 2010b), or making a bad impression (Antony & Swinson, 2000). SAD typically lasts for 12 or more years (Grant et al., 2005), has low remission rates (Massion et al., 2002), and high depression comorbidity (Stein & Stein, 2008). The associated social isolation and high rates of comorbid depression may explain some of the increased suicide risk for patients with SAD relative to those without (Thibodeau, Welch, Sareen, & Asmundson, 2013), wherein 35% contemplate suicide regularly and 14% attempt suicide (Cougle, Keough, Riccardi, & Sachs-Ericsson, 2009).

Models of SAD emphasize the central role of attentional processes and suggest that (1) heightened self-focused attention and (2) vigilance for external social threat cues influence the development and maintenance of SAD (e.g., Heimberg et al., 2010, Hofmann, 2007). People with SAD more rapidly engage with and spend more time attending to external social threat cues and emotional faces in anxiety-provoking social situations (Asmundson and Stein, 1994, Chen et al., 2002, Lee and Telch, 2008), and demonstrate biases in interpretation, attention, and imagery relative to non-anxious individuals (Hirsch & Clark, 2004). Together, these attentional processes may narrow attention, interfere with beneficial processing, and maintain SAD.

Participants in a dot-probe protocol observe a screen on which randomly paired stimuli are presented (for ∼500 ms), one stimulus above the other. One stimulus is neutral (e.g., the word table) and the other is associated with threat (e.g., the word snake). After the stimuli presentation, a probe appears in a location (top/bottom) corresponding to where one of the stimuli appeared. Participants press a key as quickly as possible to indicate the probe location (top/bottom). People with clinically significant anxiety respond faster when probes appear in the location of threat stimuli related to their anxiety (i.e., congruent) versus the location of neutral stimuli (i.e., incongruent), regardless of location (top/bottom). In contrast, people without clinically significant anxiety (i.e., controls) respond comparably to threat and neutral words, producing no difference in response times across congruent, incongruent, and neutral trials (i.e., two neutral words presented).

Modifying attentional biases using an adapted dot-probe protocol as a treatment for social anxiety has received increasing interest (Koster et al., 2009, MacLeod et al., 2002). The adapted protocol involves two conditions: (1) the Attention Control Condition (ACC), in which neutral and threat stimuli are replaced by probes with equal frequency, and (2) the Attentional Modification Condition (AMC), in which the probe consistently replaces neutral stimuli. In the AMC participants implicitly learn to direct their attention away from the threat stimulus to detect and respond to the probe, which is thought to modify the attentional bias and reduce symptoms. In one of the original studies exploring the adapted dot-probe protocol as a treatment for SAD symptoms (Schmidt, Richey, Buckner, & Timpano, 2009), participants in the AMC (n = 18), but not in the ACC (n = 18), reported a significant reduction in SAD symptoms (i.e., p < .01; d > .50). Since then, several studies have provided additional support for AMC as a method to reduce SAD symptoms (Amir et al., 2009b, Amir et al., 2011, Amir et al., 2008, Schmidt et al., 2009). Training attention away from threat appears more effective for reducing social anxiety than training attention toward threat, neutral, or positive stimuli (Heeren et al., 2011, Heeren et al., 2012); however, studies have also found evidence that the AMC and ACC can both reduce symptoms, with no significant differences in those reductions between the conditions (e.g., Enock et al., 2014, Heeren et al., 2015, Julian et al., 2012).

Much of the attention training research with social anxiety has used a static set of pre-selected word stimuli to assess for the attentional biases, but then used pictures of faces as the stimuli for modifying any such biases (Amir et al., 2009b, Heeren et al., 2015). The intent was to avoid experimental conflation of repeated exposure to the stimuli with changes in attentional biases. That said, related research supporting the success of AMC relative to ACC for generalized anxiety disorder symptoms used words for both assessment and training (Amir, Beard, Cobb, & Bomyea, 2009), with those researchers recommending research exploring the use of only words for SAD as well (Amir et al., 2011). Furthermore, tailored stimuli, rather than a pre-selected static set, should be more salient and therein produce stronger effect sizes.

Part of the appeal associated with the attention modification protocols involves the potential for wide spread cost-effective dissemination through the Internet. Indeed, such administration could be particularly suited to persons with social anxiety. At least four separate randomized controlled trials have administered the attention modification protocol remotely via the Internet to samples diagnosed with SAD found similar results (Boettcher et al., 2012, Carlbring et al., 2012, Neubauer et al., 2013, Rapee et al., 2013); however, in all cases participants in both the AMC and ACC reported social anxiety symptom reductions of small to large effects sizes with no statistically significant differences between conditions. Furthermore, the studies did not report robust evidence of attentional biases before or after treatment, changes in an attentional bias over the course of treatment, or a relationship between attentional bias and symptom changes.

A recent meta-analysis indicated that attentional biases were much smaller for studies administering the protocols through the Internet than for those administered in laboratories (Mogoase, David, & Koster, 2014). Nonetheless, the range of effect sizes for symptom changes was quite large for Internet studies (i.e., Hedges g = .05–.97) and for laboratory studies (i.e., Hedges g = .02–.82). The authors of the meta-analysis noted “the available evidence regarding ABM clinical utility [outside] the laboratory is currently limited” (Mogoase et al., 2014, p. 18) and recommended additional research exploring Internet administrations that include extended follow-up assessments (e.g., 4+ months).

The current randomized controlled study was designed to: (1) replicate and extend the initial findings presented by Schmidt et al. (2009) and Amir et al. (2008) by using word stimuli [in line with evidence from Heeren et al. (2015), Mogoase et al. (2014), and Rapee et al. (2013)] that were idiosyncratically selected; (2) evaluate the comparative impact of participation in a laboratory setting relative to remotely at home, adding to the currently limited literature; and (3) provide extended follow-up assessments evaluating the endurance of the AMC changes (i.e., at 4 and 8 months). There were five hypothesized outcomes. First, participants completing the AMC in the laboratory were expected to report significant reductions in SAD symptoms relative to participants completing the ACC in the laboratory. Second, participants completing the AMC remotely were expected to report significant reductions in SAD symptoms relative to participants completing the ACC remotely. Third, participants completing the AMC in the laboratory were expected to report greater SAD symptom reductions relative to those participating remotely. The additional symptom reduction was expected in the laboratory condition because it was thought of as an additive exposure to social situations. Fourth, participants in the AMC were expected to maintain symptom changes at 4 and 8 months after participation relative to participants in the ACC. Fifth, participants showing the greatest change in attentional biases in AMC were expected to show the greatest SAD symptom reduction, based on initial theory suggesting that AMC protocols lead to symptom improvement via change in attentional bias (Bar-Haim, 2010, Clarke et al., 2014).

Section snippets

Participants

Participants included treatment-seeking community members (n = 108; 45 men [Mage = 34.22; SD = 11.77] and 63 women [Mage = 36.92; SD = 14.02]) recruited through print and social media. The advertisements solicited potential participants from people who self-identified as having symptoms of social anxiety and were interested in participating in a computer treatment study. Previous research demonstrated large effect sizes for differences in reported symptom reductions between the AMC and ACC groups with

Sample characteristics

Descriptive statistics of the dependent variables for each group are presented in Table 1. There were no statistically significant differences between participants who discontinued after baseline, discontinued after post-treatment, discontinued after 4-month follow-up, and those who completed the 8-month follow-up on age, F(3, 104) = 2.08, p = .11, η2 = .06, years with significant SAD symptoms, F(3, 104) = 1.41, p = .25, η2 = .04, the SIPS total score, F(3, 104) = .97, p = .41, η2 = .03, the CES-D, F(3, 104) = 

Discussion

The current study was designed to replicate and extend previous findings that supported using AMC, but not ACC, to reduce SAD symptoms (Amir et al., 2008, Schmidt et al., 2009). The study also addresses recommendations from a recent meta-analysis that more research assessing remote administration of AMC is needed (Mogoase et al., 2014). The current design used word stimuli (in line with Rapee et al., 2013) that were idiosyncratically selected to ensure individual relevance, evaluated the

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank the Saskatchewan Health Research Foundation for making this research possible through a New Investigator Establishment Grant and a New Investigator Equipment Grant (SHRF2456) awarded to R. N. Carleton.

References (65)

  • A. Heeren et al.

    Attention training toward and away from threat in social phobia: Effects on subjective, behavioral, and physiological measures of anxiety

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (2012)
  • C.R. Hirsch et al.

    Information-processing bias in social phobia

    Clinical Psychology Review

    (2004)
  • K. Julian et al.

    Attention training to reduce attention bias and social stressor reactivity: An attempt to replicate and extend previous findings

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (2012)
  • H.J. Lee et al.

    Attentional biases in social anxiety: An investigation using the inattentional blindness paradigm

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (2008)
  • C. MacLeod et al.

    Anxiety and the selective processing of emotional information: Mediating roles of awareness, trait and state variables, and personal relevance of stimu

    Behaviour research and therapy

    (1992)
  • J.I. Mattia et al.

    The revised Stroop color-naming task in social phobics

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (1993)
  • A.R. Menatti et al.

    The Social Interaction Phobia Scale: Continued support for the psychometric validity of the SIPS using clinical and non-clinical samples

    Journal of Anxiety Disorders

    (2015)
  • K. Mogg et al.

    Selective orienting of attention to masked threat faces in social anxiety

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (2002)
  • K. Neubauer et al.

    Internet-delivered attention modification training as a treatment for social phobia: A randomized controlled trial

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (2013)
  • R.M. Rapee et al.

    Integrating cognitive bias modification into a standard cognitive behavioural treatment package for social phobia: A randomized controlled trial

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (2013)
  • M.B. Stein et al.

    Social anxiety disorder

    Lancet

    (2008)
  • J.W. Weeks et al.

    “Social Anxiety Disorder Carved at its Joints”: Evidence for the taxonicity of social anxiety disorder

    Journal of Anxiety Disorders

    (2010)
  • J.W. Weeks et al.

    The Fear of Positive Evaluation Scale: Assessing a proposed cognitive component of social anxiety

    Journal of Anxiety Disorders

    (2008)
  • M.C. Zanarini et al.

    Attainment and maintenance of reliability of axis I and II disorders over the course of a longitudinal study

    Comprehensive Psychiatry

    (2001)
  • American Psychiatric Association

    Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th text revision ed.)

    (2000)
  • American Psychiatric Association

    Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders

    (2013)
  • N. Amir et al.

    Attention modification program in individuals with generalized anxiety disorder

    Journal of Abnormal Psychology

    (2009)
  • N. Amir et al.

    Attention training in individuals with generalized social phobia: A randomized controlled trial

    Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology

    (2009)
  • N. Amir et al.

    Predictors of response to an attention modification program in generalized social phobia

    Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology

    (2011)
  • N. Amir et al.

    The effect of a single-session attention modification program on response to a public-speaking challenge in socially anxious individuals

    Journal of Abnormal Psychology

    (2008)
  • M.M. Antony et al.

    The shyness and social anxiety workbook: Proven techniques for overcoming your fears.

    (2000)
  • L.L.B. Barnes et al.

    Reliability generalization of scores on the Spielberger State-Trait Anxiety Inventory

    Educational and Psychological Measurement

    (2002)
  • Cited by (22)

    • Insula activation is modulated by attention shifting in social anxiety disorder

      2018, Journal of Anxiety Disorders
      Citation Excerpt :

      Despite considerable early promise, there have now been a number of studies showing no specific benefits from attention training, relative to control conditions (Carleton et al., 2015; Carleton, Teale Sapach, Oriet, & LeBouthillier, 2017; Clerkin, Magee, Wells, Beard, & Barnett, 2016; Fitzgerald, Rawdon, & Dooley, 2016; Heeren, Coussement, & McNally, 2016; Heeren, Mogoaşe, McNally et al., 2015; Pergamin-Hight, Pine, Fox, & Bar-Haim, 2016; Yao, Yu, Qian, & Li, 2015). Some of these studies do report improvements in general cognitive processes, like attention and working memory (Heeren et al., 2016; Heeren, Mogoaşe, McNally et al., 2015), and most reported reductions in observed and/or self-reported anxiety symptoms over time (Carleton et al., 2015, 2017; Clerkin et al., 2016; Heeren et al., 2016; Heeren, Mogoaşe, McNally et al., 2015; Pergamin-Hight et al., 2016; Yao et al., 2015). However, when reported, symptom improvements were not significantly different between active training and control paradigms, suggesting that practice effects may have been driving these changes.

    • Cognitive bias modification of interpretation in children with social anxiety disorder

      2017, Journal of Anxiety Disorders
      Citation Excerpt :

      Recent applications of CBM-I with children have led to mixed results. On the basis of a meta-analysis of CBM (of attention and interpretation) interventions for mental health problems in children, Cristea, Mogoase, David and Cuijpers (2015) concluded that while CBM appeared to bring about significant changes in interpretation biases, these shifts did not translate to changes in child symptoms of anxiety, depression or general mental health (consistent with recent studies of Attention Bias Modification in the context of social anxiety; Carleton et al., 2015; Heeren, Mogoaşe, McNally, Schmitz & Philippot, 2015; Yao, Yu, Qian & Li, 2015). However, interpretation measures were collapsed to include both controlled in-lab experiments and real-life, ecologically valid measures, leaving the extent to which interpretation bias changed somewhat unclear.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text