Interpretation bias modification for youth and their parents: A novel treatment for early adolescent social anxiety

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.janxdis.2014.09.011Get rights and content

Abstract

Social anxiety is the most prevalent anxiety disorder of late adolescence, yet current treatments reach only a minority of youth with the disorder. Effective and easy-to-disseminate treatments are needed. This study pilot tested the efficacy of a novel, online cognitive bias modification for interpretation (CBM-I) intervention for socially anxious youth and their parents. The CBM-I intervention targeted cognitive biases associated with early adolescents’ maladaptive beliefs regarding social situations, and with parents’ intrusive behavior, both of which have been theoretically linked with the maintenance of social anxiety in youth. To investigate the efficacy of intervening with parents and/or children, clinically diagnosed early adolescents (ages 10–15; N = 18) and their mothers were randomly assigned to one of three conditions: the first targeted early adolescents’ cognitive biases related to social anxiety (Child-only condition); the second targeted parents’ biases associated with intrusive behavior (Parent-only condition); and the third targeted both youth and parents’ biases in tandem (Combo condition). The use of a multiple baseline design allowed for the efficient assessment of causal links between the intervention and reduction in social anxiety symptoms in youth. Results provided converging evidence indicating modest support for the efficacy of CBM-I, with no reliable differences across conditions. Taken together, results suggest that online CBM-I with anxious youth and/or their parents holds promise as an effective and easily administered component of treatment for child social anxiety that deserves further evaluation in a larger trial.

Introduction

Social anxiety disorder is the most prevalent anxiety disorder of late adolescence and adulthood (Ollendick & Hirshfeld-Becker, 2002), affecting up to 15% of teenagers in the United States (Heimberg, Stein, Hiripi, & Kessler, 2000). Social anxiety typically runs a chronic course over the lifespan (Keller, 2003) and is linked to poor long-term outcomes, such as major depression (Hayward et al., 2000), academic underachievement, and substance abuse (DeWitt, McDonald, & Offord, 1999). Yet, a mere 25% of youth with social anxiety have access to any intervention (Essau, Conradt, & Petermann, 1999). Moreover, among the minority of socially anxious youth who do receive the current gold standard treatment, cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT), approximately 40% fail to demonstrate a clinically significant treatment response (Kendall, Settipani, & Cummings, 2012). Clearly, additional treatments are needed—both to reach underserved populations and to increase the efficacy of CBT.

Cognitive models of social anxiety (Clark and Wells, 1995, Rapee and Heimberg, 1997, Schreiber et al., 2012) posit that, in social contexts, multiple cognitive biases are activated for socially anxious individuals. For example, their attention becomes especially self-focused, and they experience greater access to negative beliefs about themselves and others’ evaluation of them. These cognitive processes may give rise to negatively biased interpretations of the many ambiguous cues that social situations present (Beard & Amir, 2008), which, in turn, amplify and/or maintain social anxiety symptoms. Empirical work corroborates the theorized connection between interpretation bias and anxiety symptoms in youth. For example, studies have shown that anxious youth tend to interpret ambiguous hypothetical stories as threatening (e.g., Bögels & Zigterman, 2000), and do so based on less information than non-anxious youth (e.g. Muris, Merckelbach, & Damsma, 2000).

The child anxiety literature also supports a role for family interactions in shaping interpretation bias. For example, two similar studies assessed clinically anxious youth's interpretation bias of ambiguous situations (Chorpita, Albano, & Barlow, 1996) and their hypothetical behavioral response (e.g., avoidance) to ambiguous situations (Barrett, Rapee, Dadds, & Ryan, 1996), both before and after discussions with family. Both studies found that anxious youth initially interpreted ambiguous scenarios more negatively than non-anxious youth, and, moreover, anxious youth's negative interpretations (Chorpita et al., 1996) and expected avoidant behaviors (Barrett et al., 1996) increased following discussions with their families. Further, Creswell and O’Connor (2006) found that the correlation between mothers’ and children's threatening interpretations of ambiguous scenarios was partially mediated by mothers’ expectations for the child's distress, suggesting that children's interpretation biases may have a “reciprocal relationship with mothers’ expectations of children's anxious cognitions” (Field, Hadwin, & Lester, 2011). These findings indicate the possibility that modifying interpretation biases at both the parent and child level could lead to reduced anxiety symptoms in youth.

Cognitive bias modification for threat interpretation (CBM-I) refers to computerized training in interpreting ambiguity in a benign way, so as to reduce threat-related interpretations and increase benign interpretations of ambiguous situations in participants’ everyday life (Mathews & Mackintosh, 2000). The adult anxiety literature has shown that directly reducing negative cognitive biases through CBM-I can lead to reductions in anxiety symptoms, highlighting a causal role for cognitive biases in anxiety and, in turn, the clinical utility of CBM-I (MacLeod and Mathews, 2012, Steinman and Teachman, 2014). Thus, several child and adolescent studies have adapted the ambiguous scenario paradigm that has commonly been used in adult CBM-I studies. This paradigm presents emotionally ambiguous scenarios that are resolved when participants complete a word fragment at the end of the scenario that assigns a benign meaning to the situation (in the positive training condition). Results suggest that youth's interpretation biases can be altered through CBM-I (see Lau, 2013), though many questions remain. For example, across studies, changes in anxiety symptoms have rarely accompanied changes in interpretation bias, and effect sizes have been small. Nonetheless, findings from the burgeoning youth CBM-I literature, taken together with those from the more established adult literature, suggest that several methodological modifications, which are applied in this study, could increase the efficacy of youth CBM-I and its ability to alter anxiety symptoms.

For example, ecologically valid training materials have been associated with larger effect sizes. Whereas early youth CBM-I studies used scenarios related to a fictional “space odyssey” theme, subsequent work (Lester, Field, & Muris, 2011) used materials involving real-life scenarios congruent with participants’ anxiety symptoms related to either animal or social fears. In addition to finding that children's threat bias decreased after positive training in both the animal and social conditions, a non-significant trend suggested that bias modification was stronger for children trained with content that was congruent with their developmentally normative fears. Thus, all CBM-I scenarios in the current study targeted beliefs theoretically and/or empirically linked to intrusive parenting or social anxiety.

Of note, one of the few studies that employed a high social anxiety (vs. unselected) sample was also one of the few youth CBM-I studies to show reductions in participants’ trait social anxiety following training (Vassilopoulos, Banerjee, & Prantzalou, 2009). However, the only published youth CBM-I study that has employed a clinically diagnosed sample did not find reductions in emotional vulnerability following training (Fu, Du, Au, & Lau, 2013). The current study's inclusion of clinically diagnosed socially anxious youth aims to provide a clearer test of the clinical utility of CBM-I for children and maximize the likelihood of observing effects.

Finally, youth CBM-I studies have typically focused treatment on only the child or adolescent, which may neglect the need to address the family context in which youth anxiety develops. In the only published youth CBM-I study to include parents (Lau, Pettit, & Creswell, 2013), CBM-I scenarios were embedded in bedtime stories that parents read to their children over three nights. Children who received the intervention not only showed positive changes in interpretation bias, they also showed a significant reduction in social anxiety symptoms, relative to control participants. While the inclusion of parents marks an exciting new direction in youth CBM-I research, we note that Lau et al. included parents as administrators of treatment, as opposed to the recipients. Given the purported role of parents in child anxiety, it is possible that administering CBM-I to both parents and children could have additive benefits.

Though the role of parenting in child anxiety has not been clearly specified, one aspect of parenting that may influence child anxiety is parental intrusiveness. McLeod, Wood, and Avny (2011) point to multiple mechanisms that may account for the positive relationship between parental intrusiveness and child anxiety. Parental intrusiveness may lower children's self-efficacy or sense of an internal locus of control, or it could block opportunities for exposure to feared stimuli, which has been strongly linked to fear reduction (Rachman, 1977). For example, some benign and developmentally appropriate situations, such as parties and class presentations, may make anxious youth fearful. In response to their child's distress, parents may enable their child's avoidance of these situations, thereby preventing the child from learning that he or she is capable of facing fears and managing anxiety.

Ollendick and Benoit's (2012) parent–child interactional model of social anxiety proposes that parents may inadvertently contribute to child social anxiety by modeling anxiety and communicating their own cognitive biases to their children, such as the overestimation of social threat and the belief that avoidance is a helpful response to perceived threat. These biases may lead to parenting practices, such as intrusive parenting, that shape and sustain child cognitive biases, which in turn maintain child social anxiety. According to this model, targeting parent cognitive biases that are thought to contribute to child anxiety (e.g., my child cannot tolerate anxiety; I will be a bad parent if I don’t stop my child's anxiety) may interrupt the transmission of anxiety from parent to child at an early point in this process.

Internet delivery of youth CBT has gained empirical support as being both feasible and efficacious (see Richardson, Stallard, & Velleman, 2010, for a review). Online youth CBM-I, on the other hand, has only been attempted once, to our knowledge (Sportel, de Hullu, de Jong, & Nauta, 2013). While the outcomes indicated only a trend for symptom reduction in the group who received CBM, it is notable that participants assigned to the Internet-based CBM condition completed more treatment sessions than those in the school-based group CBT condition, suggesting that Internet CBM is at least feasible and does not lead to unusually high attrition.

Though Sportel et al. (2013) intervened with a community sample, Internet delivery of CBM-I may be especially promising for socially anxious youth. Many socially anxious youth wish to avoid social situations (including therapy), yet they appear to use the Internet at least as much as youth without social anxiety (e.g., Mazalin & Moore, 2004). CBM-I may also be preferable to CBT for these youth. Whereas online CBT typically requires 30–60 min per session (Richardson et al., 2010), CBM-I sessions are typically designed to be completed in 20 min or less. Moreover, CBT demands introspection and sharing of explicit insight into thoughts and feelings (which can be challenging for socially reticent youth); CBM-I provides participants with short scenarios to be resolved in a game-like format, without explicitly requiring introspection.

We designed the present intervention for socially anxious youth who were 10–15-years-old for several reasons. First, social anxiety most commonly emerges between early and mid-adolescence (e.g., ages 10–16; Beidel, 1998, Mesa et al., 2011) and increases with age. By including the lower end of this age range, we hoped to improve outcomes for youth with early onset of symptoms, which has been empirically linked to chronicity (DeWitt et al., 1999). Second, this period of social anxiety's emergence maps on to both youth's growing capacity for independence, and the normative attenuation of parent involvement in aspects of their children's everyday functioning. Thus, parents’ continuation of intrusive behaviors at this stage in particular may limit the development of their children's independence.

To build on the promising but mixed youth CBM-I literature, the current study included several methodological modifications: (1) creating externally valid, developmentally informed materials that engage our pre-adolescent participants; (2) intervening with a clinically diagnosed sample; (3) increasing the number of CBM-I sessions to eight, a number found to be effective in the adult literature (e.g., Beard & Amir, 2008) but rarely found in the youth literature; (4) administering treatment online, as opposed to in the lab; and perhaps most importantly, (5) administering treatment to both anxious children and their parents.

Answering calls for more idiographic research in clinical science (Barlow & Nock, 2009), a multiple baseline design examined whether the intervention is efficacious in reducing symptoms of social anxiety. Idiographic experiment designs, such as multiple baseline designs, are “ideally suited for use by psychological scientists,” because they can efficiently and with few participants “provide strong evidence of causal relations between variables” (Barlow & Nock, 2009, p. 20). In multiple baseline designs, participants are repeatedly assessed on key variables until stable responding is established. Following this baseline period, the intervention is introduced. If changes in key variables occur only when the intervention is introduced, the change can be attributed to the intervention. For this reason, multiple baseline designs have been called a “critical link” to the judicious allocation of time and funds toward subsequent, larger randomized controlled trials (Moras, Telfer, & Barlow, 1993, p. 412). Given the current intervention has not been previously been tested, our multiple baseline design was ideal to gather useful information about its likely efficacy, which can be applied to a more traditional, larger-scale clinical trial.

To investigate whether it is most efficacious to intervene with parents and/or children, youth and their mothers were randomly assigned to one of three conditions: one targeted early adolescents’ cognitive biases related to social anxiety (Child-only condition); the second targeted parents’ biases associated with intrusive behavior (Parent-only condition); and the third targeted both youth and parents’ biases in tandem (Combo condition). We hypothesized that youth in all conditions would adhere to the study protocol (as indicated by successful completion of the eight training sessions and the assessment battery), and that they would show reduced negative interpretation bias, increased positive interpretation bias, and significant improvements in social anxiety symptoms. Given research suggesting that both children's and parents’ beliefs may maintain child social anxiety (see Negreiros & Miller, 2014), addressing both sets of maladaptive beliefs may exert additive effects on reducing child anxiety symptoms. Thus, we hypothesized that participants in the Combo condition would improve the most.

Section snippets

Participants

Participants were recruited from suburban and rural areas of the Southeastern United States through flyers, advertisements, and social networking. Potential parent participants completed a semi-structured diagnostic phone screen. If the phone screen suggested a probable child social anxiety disorder diagnosis, then child verbal consent was obtained via phone, and both the parent and child were invited to an intake assessment at the clinic. Study inclusion criteria were a diagnosis of child

Adherence

All participants completed the entire study protocol, indicating overall excellent adherence. Participants were told that measures and/or sessions should be completed every three days, but that this timeline could be somewhat flexible in order to accommodate family's busy schedules. The mean (SD) and modal number of days between measures was 4.5 (2.84) and 3, respectively, suggesting that most measures and/or sessions were completed every three days, with some variability2

Discussion

The primary goals of this pilot study were to evaluate the feasibility and preliminary efficacy of CBM-I for socially anxious youth and their parents. To test whether including parents in treatment would improve outcomes in youth, participants were randomized to three conditions that included children only, parents only, or both in treatment (i.e., the Combo condition). It was hypothesized that all youth would show reductions in negative interpretation bias, increases in positive interpretation

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to Daniel Martin and Karen Grace Martin for their statistical consultation on this project, as well as Kristy Benoit and Stephanos Vassilopoulos for sharing materials from their studies to help with the development of our training program. We would also like to thank the Teachman Program for Anxiety, Cognition, and Treatment (PACT) laboratory for their feedback on this project. This research was supported in part by an NIH grant no. R01AG033033 to Bethany Teachman. Note:

References (63)

  • J.S. March et al.

    A randomized controlled trial of venlafaxine ER versus placebo in pediatric social anxiety disorder

    Biological Psychiatry

    (2007)
  • B.D. McLeod et al.

    Examining the association between parenting and childhood anxiety: a meta-analysis

    Clinical Psychology Review

    (2007)
  • T.H. Ollendick et al.

    The developmental psychopathology of social anxiety disorder

    Biological Psychiatry

    (2002)
  • B.S. Parsonson et al.

    The analysis and presentation of graphic data

  • S. Rachman

    The conditioning theory of fear-acquisition: a critical examination

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (1977)
  • R.M. Rapee et al.

    Reliabiltiy of the DSM-III-R childhood anxiety disorders using structured interview: interrater and parent–child agreement

    Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry

    (1994)
  • R.M. Rapee et al.

    A cognitive-behavioral model of social phobia

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (1997)
  • L. Seligman et al.

    Cognitive behavioral therapy for anxiety disorders in youth

    Child and Adolescent Psychiatry Clinics of North America

    (2011)
  • S.A. Steinman et al.

    Modifying interpretations among individuals high in anxiety sensitivity

    Journal of Anxiety Disorders

    (2010)
  • S.P. Vassilopoulos et al.

    Experimental modification of interpretation bias in socially anxious children: changes in interpretation, anticipated interpersonal anxiety, and social anxiety symptoms

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (2009)
  • D.H. Barlow et al.

    Why can’t we be more idiographic in our research?

    Psychological Science

    (2009)
  • P.M. Barrett et al.

    Family enhancement of cognitive style in anxious and aggressive children

    Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology

    (1996)
  • K.E. Benoit

    Interpretation bias in anxious mothers and their children: can interpretation modification affect the intergenerational transmission of anxiety?

    (2013)
  • K.D. Becker et al.

    Maternal anxiety, behaviors, and expectations during a behavioral task: relation to children's self-evaluations

    Child Psychiatry and Human Development

    (2011)
  • D.C. Beidel

    Social anxiety disorder: etiology and early clinical presentation

    The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry

    (1998)
  • S.M. Bögels et al.

    Dysfunctional cognitions in children with social phobia, separation anxiety disorder, and generalized anxiety disorder

    Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology

    (2000)
  • B. Chorpita

    Modular cognitive-behavioral therapy for childhood anxiety disorders: guides to individual evidence base treatment

    (2006)
  • B. Chorpita et al.

    Cognitive processing in children: relation to anxiety and family influences

    Journal of Clinical Child Psychology

    (1996)
  • D. Clark et al.

    A cognitive model of social phobia

  • C. Creswell et al.

    Anxious cognitions in children: an exploration of associations and mediators

    British Journal of Developmental Psychology

    (2006)
  • D.J. DeWitt et al.

    Childhood stress and symptoms of drug dependence in adolescence and early adulthood: social phobia as a mediator

    American Journal of Orthopsychiatry

    (1999)
  • Cited by (28)

    • A Pilot Feasibility Open Trial of an Interpretation Bias Intervention for Parents of Anxious Children

      2022, Cognitive and Behavioral Practice
      Citation Excerpt :

      Such questions will be important to answer to create robust parent interpretation interventions. Additionally, future work should examine the potential added benefit of targeting parent and child cognitive bias simultaneously (e.g., Reuland & Teachman, 2014). Waitlists for child anxiety treatment, even in provider-rich regions, are extremely long (e.g., 6 months).

    • The impact of parent-only interventions on child anxiety: A systematic review and meta-analysis

      2022, Journal of Affective Disorders
      Citation Excerpt :

      The level of heterogeneity was very low (Q = 5.11, df = 5, I2 = 2%) (Fig. 7). Six studies reporting child-rated outcomes were not included in any meta-analysis because either mean scores or information regarding the sample size of each condition was not provided (Brown et al., 2017; Leong et al., 2009; Özyurt et al., 2016; Reuland and Teachman, 2014; Salari et al., 2018; Thienemann et al., 2006). Calculated effect sizes for three of the four single group studies were large (Table 6), although again the sample sizes in these studies were small (Eisen et al., 2008; Esbjørn et al., 2016; Raleigh, 2001).

    • A systematic review of treatments targeting cognitive biases in socially anxious adolescents: Special Section on “Translational and Neuroscience Studies in Affective Disorders” Section Editor, Maria Nobile MD, PhD

      2020, Journal of Affective Disorders
      Citation Excerpt :

      Authors did not examine possible effects on anxiety symptoms, making it difficult to ascertain whether the reported change in cognitive bias was clinically meaningful. Reuland and Teachman (2014) assigned adolescents with SAD and their mothers to 1 of 3 online interventions: (i) CBMT-I for the child only (n = =6), targeting cognitive biases associated with adolescents’ maladaptive beliefs about social situations; (ii) CBMT-I for the parent only (n = =5), targeting biases known to drive intrusive parenting practices (e.g., my child cannot tolerate anxiety; I will be a bad parent if I don't stop my child's anxiety); and (iii) CBMT-I targeting both youth and parents’ biases in combination (n = =6). Larger effect sizes were seen for change in parent compared with child interpretation bias, with no substantial differences in effect sizes between positive and negative bias, and no significant differences across conditions.

    • Thinking anxious, feeling anxious, or both? Cognitive bias moderates the relationship between anxiety disorder status and sympathetic arousal in youth

      2017, Journal of Anxiety Disorders
      Citation Excerpt :

      Of note, in the last two decades, cognitive bias modification (CBM) interventions have been used to directly intervene upon attention and interpretation biases with the goal of reducing anxiety symptoms (for reviews, see Beard, Sawyer, & Hofmann, 2012; Hakamata, Lissek, Bar-Haim, & Britton, 2010; Hallion & Ruscio, 2011; Menne-Lothmann, Viechtbauer, & Höhn, 2014). In addition to reducing anxiety symptoms in clinically anxious adolescents (Reuland & Teachman, 2014), CBM for interpretation bias in particular has been shown to reduce self-reported anxiety during psychological challenge (Lau, Belli, & Chopra, 2013) and life stress (Telman, Holmes, & Lau, 2013) in non-diagnosed youth samples. Physiological arousal, or the nervous system’s response to real or perceive threat, is also an associated feature of anxiety.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text