Elsevier

Journal of Affective Disorders

Volume 189, 1 January 2016, Pages 233-239
Journal of Affective Disorders

Research report
Transgenerational effects of maternal depression on affect recognition in children

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2015.09.051Get rights and content

Highlights

  • In remission, mothers with depression show a negative processing bias.

  • Their children show parallel alterations in their affect recognition.

  • Mothers with remitted depression with or without history of abuse do not differ.

Abstract

Background

The association between maternal depression and adverse emotional and behavioral outcomes in children is well established. One associated factor might be altered affect recognition which may be transmitted transgenerationally. Individuals with history of depression show biased recognition of sadness. Our aim was to investigate parallels in maternal and children's affect recognition with remitted depressed mothers.

Methods

60 Mother–child dyads completed an affect recognition morphing task. We examined two groups of remitted depressed mothers, with and without history of physical or sexual abuse, and a group of healthy mothers without history of physical or sexual abuse. Children were between 5 and 12 years old.

Results

Across groups, mothers identified happy faces fastest. Mothers with remitted depression showed a higher accuracy and response bias for sadness. We found corresponding results in their children. Maternal and children's bias and accuracy for sadness were positively correlated. Effects of remitted depression were found independent of maternal history of physical or sexual abuse.

Limitations

Our sample size was relatively small and further longitudinal research is needed to investigate how maternal and children's affect recognition are associated with behavioral and emotional outcomes in the long term.

Conclusions

Our data suggest a negative processing bias in mothers with remitted depression which might represent both the perpetuation of and vulnerability to depression. Children of remitted depressed mothers appear to be exposed to this processing bias outside acute depressive episodes. This may promote the development of a corresponding processing bias in the children and could make children of depressed mothers more vulnerable to depressive disorders themselves.

Introduction

There is ample empirical evidence for an association between maternal depression and a range of adverse child behavioral (Lee and Gotlib, 1991) and emotional outcomes (for review, see: Beardslee et al., 2011, Goodman et al., 2011). Children of depressed parents often show diminished affect regulation and cognitive and language functioning when their mothers are depressed (e.g., Carter et al., 2001; Cicchetti et al., 1998; Rohde et al., 2005). They are at risk for developing depressive disorders themselves (Sim and England, 2009) and other internalizing and externalizing disorders (Lyons-Ruth et al., 1997). However, the factors contributing to the transgenerational transmission of depression have not been fully elucidated. One psychological factor contributing to the risk of transmission might be altered affect recognition in mothers, such as difficulties in correctly interpreting and responding to facial expressions.

In adults, a number of studies have investigated alterations in facial affect recognition in depression. These studies consistently showed an increased accuracy of, and increased response bias for sad faces (Arteche et al., 2011, Gollan et al., 2010, Gollan et al., 2008, Gur et al., 1992, Hale, 1998, Lee et al., 2008, Mandal and Bhattacharya, 1985, Sterzer et al., 2011, Surguladze et al., 2004). These emotional-cognitive deficits seem to persist during remission (Bhagwagar and Cowen, 2008, Bouhuys et al., 1999, Fuhr et al., 2014, Joormann and Gotlib, 2007, Lange et al., 2012). The question arises whether biased affect recognition might be transmitted from affected mothers to their children. Investigations addressing this question have only recently begun and suggest that alterations in affect recognition may also exist in children of depressed mothers. The alterations seem mainly to concern sad affects (Joormann et al., 2010; Lopez‐Duran et al., 2013). To the best of our knowledge, to date no studies exist that have directly investigated parallels in affect recognition in mother–child dyads.

We here studied affect recognition in both mothers with remitted depression and their children to address the question of whether biased affect recognition in remitted depressed mothers corresponds with respective alterations in their children.

Patients with depression often report a history of traumatization, such as physical or sexual abuse (Heim et al., 2008, Lizardi et al., 1995, McCabe et al., 2009). Based on neurobiological (Heim et al., 2008, Vythilingam et al., 2002) and treatment studies (Nemeroff et al., 2003), it has been suggested that depressed patients with and without trauma represent two distinct subgroups of depression. This distinction may be critical for the investigation of affect recognition, as history of trauma has – independently of depression - been associated with altered affect recognition. However, these alterations seem to concern biased anger and fear processing mainly (Dannlowski et al., 2012, Poljac et al., 2011, Rauch et al., 2000, Shin et al., 2005, van Harmelen et al., 2010). To account for effects of trauma, we here distinguished between remitted depressed mothers with and without history of trauma (i.e., physical or sexual abuse).

We tested two hypotheses in the present study. First, we predicted that mothers with remitted depression would exhibit higher accuracy and a response bias for sad faces. Second, we hypothesized that children of remitted depressed mothers would show corresponding alterations.

To study these hypotheses we performed an affect recognition task in mothers with remitted depression and their children. We tested for parallels between mothers and children both at group and at dyadic level. At group level, we investigated whether group differences observed between depression and control mothers were analogously found in children. At dyadic level, we examined whether maternal and children's abilities in affect recognition for sad facial expressions were correlated.

Section snippets

Participants

The current study involved 81 mothers (N=23 healthy controls, N=20 remitted depression only, N=38 remitted depression with trauma) and their children (n=16 of healthy control mothers, n=15 of remitted depressed mothers, n=29 of remitted depressed mothers with history of trauma). Mean time elapsed since last depressive episode was 54.7 months (range: four to 318 months). Mean number of depressive episodes was 2.2 (range: one to six). The majority (84.0%) of remitted depressed mothers had

Response times

Mothers: In mothers, we did not find a main effect of group (F(2,69)=2.619, p>.05; η2=.07), but the main effect of affect was significant (F(3,207)=446.002, p≤.001; η2=.87): participants identified happy faces earlier compared to the other affects. This was consistent across groups indicated by a non-significant group by affect interaction (F(6,207)=1.201, p>.05; η2=.034).

Children: In children, we did not find a main effect of group (F(2,52)=0.007, p>.05; η2=.00), but the main effect of affect

Discussion

This present study used a morphing task to investigate facial affect recognition in remitted depressed mothers and their children. We found striking parallels between mothers and children both at group and at dyadic level. At group level, mothers with depression in remission showed a negative processing bias during affect recognition, which was paralleled in their children who showed a similar sadness bias. At dyadic level, we found a positive correlation between maternal and child sadness

Limitations

A number of limitations of the present study may be considered. First, it is acknowledged that the total number of children in each group was relatively small (control: n=14; depression only: n=14; depression & trauma: n=27) which is due to the challenge of recruiting mother–child dyads fulfilling all inclusion criteria. Second, our stimulus material consisted of (unfamiliar) adult faces only. In doing so, we relied on an established affect recognition task (Domes et al., 2008). It will be

Conclusions

Our findings may have clinical implications for both mothers and children. Even during remission, mothers with depression showed altered affect recognition, particularly concerning sad facial expressions. This persistent processing bias might contribute to the onset of future depressive episodes in the mothers and through social learning also impact on the offspring. Indeed, our cross-sectional and correlational data suggest that the bias is transmitted to the children. In the next generation,

Financial support

This work was partially supported by the German Federal Ministry of Education and Research (BMBF; Grant number: 01KR1207C; www.ubica.de); and the German Research Foundation (DFG; Grant number: BE2611/2-1).

Conflict of interest

None.

Ethical standards

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008.

References (62)

  • N. Just et al.

    Remitted depression studies as tests of the cognitive vulnerability hypotheses of depression onset: A critique and conceptual analysis

    Clin. Psychol. Rev.

    (2001)
  • C. Lange et al.

    Affective set-shifting deficits in patients with major depression in remission

    J. Psychiatr. Res.

    (2012)
  • Y. Lecrubier et al.

    The Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (MINI). A short diagnostic structured interview: reliability and validity according to the CIDI

    Eur. Psychiatry

    (1997)
  • B.-T. Lee et al.

    Neural correlates of affective processing in response to sad and angry facial stimuli in patients with major depressive disorder

    Prog. Neuro-Psychopharmacol. Biol. Psychiatry

    (2008)
  • A. Lischke et al.

    Intranasal oxytocin enhances emotion recognition from dynamic facial expressions and leaves eye-gaze unaffected

    Psychoneuroendocrinology

    (2012)
  • E. Poljac et al.

    Reduced recognition of fear and sadness in post-traumatic stress disorder

    Cortex

    (2011)
  • S.L. Rauch et al.

    Exaggerated amygdala response to masked facial stimuli in posttraumatic stress disorder: a functional MRI study

    Biol. Psychiatry

    (2000)
  • A.L. van Harmelen et al.

    Reduced medial prefrontal cortex volume in adults reporting childhood emotional maltreatment

    Biol. Psychiatry

    (2010)
  • M.H. Van Ijzendoorn et al.

    A sniff of trust: meta-analysis of the effects of intranasal oxytocin administration on face recognition, trust to in-group, and trust to out-group

    Psychoneuroendocrinology

    (2012)
  • H. Abdi

    The greenhouse–geisser correction

  • Y. Apter-Levy et al.

    Impact of maternal depression across the first 6 years of life on the child’s mental health, social engagement, and empathy: the moderating role of oxytocin

    Am. J. Psychiatry

    (2014)
  • A.T. Beck

    Thinking and depression: II. Theory and therapy

    Arch. Gen. Psychiatry

    (1964)
  • A.T. Beck

    Cognitive models of depression

  • Z. Bhagwagar et al.

    ‘It's not over when it's over’: persistent neurobiological abnormalities in recovered depressed patients.

    Psychol. Med.

    (2008)
  • A. Bifulco et al.

    Childhood Experience of Care and Abuse (CECA): a retrospective interview measure

    J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry

    (1994)
  • A.L. Bouhuys et al.

    Depressed patients' perceptions of facial emotions in depressed and remitted states are associated with relapse: a longitudinal study

    J. Nerv. Ment. Dis.

    (1999)
  • D. Cicchetti et al.

    Maternal depressive disorder and contextual risk: contributions to the development of attachment insecurity and behavior problems in toddlerhood

    Dev. Psychopathol.

    (1998)
  • G. Domes et al.

    J. Pers. Disord.

    (2008)
  • G. Downey et al.

    Children of depressed parents: an integrative review

    Psychol. Bull.

    (1990)
  • S. Geisser et al.

    An extension of box's results on the use of the $ F $ distribution in multivariate analysis

    Ann. Math. Stat.

    (1958)
  • J.K. Gollan et al.

    How do depressed and healthy adults interpret nuanced facial expressions?

    J. Abnorm. Psychol.

    (2010)
  • Cited by (22)

    • Epigenetic variation at the SLC6A4 gene promoter in mother–child pairs with major depressive disorder

      2019, Journal of Affective Disorders
      Citation Excerpt :

      Maternal depression may affect the mother–child interaction, with negative effects on child development, resulting in adverse emotional and behavioral outcomes (Kluczniok et al., 2016; Mendes et al., 2012).

    • Neuroticism and extraversion mediate the relationship between having a sibling with developmental disabilities and anxiety and depression symptoms

      2019, Journal of Affective Disorders
      Citation Excerpt :

      Rosenman and Rodgers (2006) found significant associations between remembered childhood adversity and high levels of Neuroticism, and that the effects of maternal depression had lasting and fundamental consequences that did not subside with age. The transgenerational transmission effect between maternal depression and negative affect in her offspring is well-established (e.g., Katz et al., 2013; Klucznoik et al., 2016) and this effect might be further exacerbated by the additional situations that the siblings already encounter, thus potentially helping to explain their higher levels of anxiety and depression symptoms. The birth order of siblings would be an interesting aspect to examine in future research; specifically, whether there are differences in personality traits dependent upon whether the sibling was born before or after the sibling with disabilities, and how this relates to their experience and their affective symptomatology.

    • Interacting and dissociable effects of alexithymia and depression on empathy

      2018, Psychiatry Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      However, a separate analysis of stimulus valence in this perspective-taking task revealed that depressed patients showed a higher accuracy rate in decoding negative mental states compared to healthy controls (Wolkenstein et al., 2011), indicating that MDD-patients show a hypervigilance to negative emotional stimuli (Joormann and Gotlib, 2010). Such negative processing bias has been observed in a variety of cognitive and emotional processes (beyond social cognition), e.g., a specific attentional and memory bias towards negative cues in MDD patients (Leppänen, 2006; Sterzer et al., 2011; Kluczniok et al., 2016; Hoffmann et al., 2016a) and could be expected to occur also in emotional empathy. When studying empathy in depression, it may be critical to consider that MDD patients may exhibit deficits in executive functioning and that these deficits may impact on empathic abilities (Thoma et al., 2011).

    • Quantifying facial expression signal and intensity use during development

      2018, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology
      Citation Excerpt :

      The low rates of fear recognition from both signal and intensity measures in our study suggest that for optimal recognition additional information is required, perhaps from several modalities. Experiential factors affecting the recognition of sadness have also been shown in studies of depressed adults (Arteche et al., 2011; Gollan, McCloskey, Hoxha, & Coccaro, 2010; Gur et al., 1992; Kluczniok et al., 2016). Precisely because measuring an individual’s prior experience of emotional expressions empirically is difficult, measuring different cohorts to inform how cultural and social experiences affect our capacity to recognize emotions is valuable.

    • Emotional availability in mothers with borderline personality disorder and mothers with remitted major depression is differently associated with psychopathology among school-aged children

      2018, Journal of Affective Disorders
      Citation Excerpt :

      Maternal emotional availability mediates the association between maternal rMDD and child psychiatric outcome. The present study was performed within the UBICA (“Understanding and Breaking the Intergenerational Cycle of Abuse”, www.ubica.de) project which investigates the effects of maternal mental disorders and history of ELM on mother-child interaction and child well-being (Dittrich et al., 2018; Fuchs et al., 2016; Kluczniok et al., 2016, 2015). Participants were recruited in Berlin between December 2012 and December 2016.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text