Elsevier

Journal of Affective Disorders

Volume 188, 1 December 2015, Pages 22-27
Journal of Affective Disorders

Research report
Transdiagnostic mechanisms in depression and anxiety: The role of rumination and attentional control

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2015.08.008Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Tested via path analysis relations among attentional control, rumination, and symptoms.

  • Poor attentional control was associated with more rumination.

  • More rumination was associated with more severe symptoms of depression and anxiety.

  • The reverse relationship (attentional control mediating) was not significant.

  • Interventions should target both attentional control and rumination.

Abstract

Background

Deficits in attentional control have been hypothesized to cause rumination, suggesting that the relationships between attentional control and clinical symptoms may be mediated in part by rumination. However, to date, no clinical study has examined these constructs transdiagnostically in a path analysis model.

Methods

Fifty-one adults presenting for treatment completed measures of self-reported attentional control, rumination, and depression and anxiety symptoms. A bias-corrected path analysis-based approach was employed to test whether indirect (i.e., mediating) effects of rumination were significantly associated with the direct effects of attentional control on depression and anxiety symptoms. Separate models for depression and anxiety symptoms were tested along with reverse models using attentional control as a proposed mediator.

Results

The relationship between attentional control and clinical symptomatology (i.e., both depression and anxiety symptoms) was mediated by rumination. Poor attentional control was associated with more rumination and consequently more severe symptoms of depression and anxiety. The reverse relationship (i.e., attentional control mediating the relationship between rumination and depression or anxiety symptoms) was not significant.

Limitations

Study design did not allow testing of temporal precedence for the mediation models. All constructs were assessed via self-report.

Conclusions

Attentional control appears to impact depression and anxiety symptoms through rumination. The pathway between poor attentional control and emotion dysregulation via rumination suggests that interventions targeting attentional control may decrease maladaptive ruminative processes, leading to improved emotion regulation and reduced clinical symptomatology. Future studies should examine the stability of this mediational relationship over time (and in the face of targeted interventions).

Introduction

Rumination, broadly defined as repetitive thinking about self-relevant negative information or one's symptoms, has historically been associated with depression. However, more recent work has revealed that rumination is a transdiagnostic construct relevant across mood, anxiety, and psychotic disorders (Hartley et al., 2014, Just and Alloy, 1997, McLaughlin and Nolen-Hoeksema, 2011, Mellings and Alden, 2000, Muris et al., 2005, Roelofs et al., 2008, Spasojevic and Alloy, 2001, Surrence et al., 2009, Wolkenstein et al., 2014). It is important to identify mechanisms underlying rumination in order to more effectively target this maladaptive style of thinking in treatment. To this end, recent theoretical models (De Raedt and Koster, 2010, Koster et al., 2011) propose that deficits in attentional control may underlie rumination, and thus serve as important treatment targets themselves.

Attentional control refers to the ability to direct attention toward or away from stimuli depending on current goals or task demands. Attentional control affects a number of related cognitive processes, such as working memory and inhibition. Koster et al. (2011) proposed that the crucial cognitive vulnerability factor leading to excessive or persistent rumination is poor attentional control, and more specifically impaired ability to disengage attention from negative thoughts. This hypothesis contrasts with previous theories that characterized rumination primarily in terms of increased attention and focus on one's symptoms and their implications (Nolen-Hoeksema, 2000, Nolen-Hoeksema and Davis, 1999, Nolen-Hoeksema and Morrow, 1991). Koster et al. (2011) suggest that individuals with good attentional control are able to disengage from negative thoughts, which allows for various emotion regulation strategies to be employed, ending the cycle of negative mood and rumination. Although Koster et al. (2011) focus exclusively on the relationship between rumination and depression, one could apply their model to explain relationships between rumination and anxiety (Mellings and Alden, 2000).

Empirical support for the theorized relationships between attentional control and rumination comes from a variety of tasks assumed to involve attentional control. For example, using a dot probe task, Donaldson et al. (2007) found that rumination was related to an attentional bias for negative words, even when controlling for depressive symptoms. Of note, several studies have shown that high trait ruminators perform more poorly than low ruminators on tasks requiring inhibition of non-valenced information (Daches et al., 2010, De Lissnyder et al., 2012, Whitmer and Banich, 2007), suggesting that the impairments in attentional control associated with rumination might not be valence-specific (however, see Tortella-Feliu et al., 2014). Self-reported attentional control has also been associated with rumination in an undergraduate sample (Fergus et al., 2012).

Although there is a wealth of data linking poor attentional control and rumination, and findings supporting their separate associations with psychopathology, there are very few studies examining these two constructs together as they relate to clinical symptoms. Furthermore, as noted in the review by Koster et al. (2011), most studies examining the relationship between rumination and attentional control have relied on undergraduate samples. To our knowledge, no study has employed mediational models to test the hypothesis that rumination mediates the relationship between attentional control and psychopathology in a clinical sample. Such data are needed as the field works to identify transdiagnostic mechanisms in order to develop more effective and targeted interventions for rumination and attentional control, as current interventions for attentional control (e.g., attention bias modification) and rumination (e.g., CBT) have room for more precision and increased efficacy in addressing these constructs.

The current study aimed to clarify the relationship between self-reported attentional control, rumination, and clinical symptomatology (e.g., anxiety and depression symptoms). Taking a transdiagnostic approach, we examined these constructs in a highly comorbid, heterogeneous, real-world patient population presenting for treatment at a partial hospital. We hypothesized that poor attentional control and higher levels of rumination would be associated with more severe depression and anxiety symptoms, and that rumination would mediate the relationship between poor attentional control and clinical symptomatology.

Section snippets

Participants

Participants were patients receiving treatment at the Behavioral Health Partial Hospital Program at McLean Hospital. The partial hospital provides brief, intensive group, and individual evidenced-based psychotherapy (e.g., Cognitive Behavioral Therapy (CBT), Dialectical Behavior Therapy (DBT)) and pharmacological treatment to patients suffering from a wide range of psychiatric disorders (principally mood, anxiety, personality, and psychotic disorders; see Björgvinsson et al., 2014 for more

Participant characteristics

Characteristics of the study sample are summarized in Table 1. Medication data were available from medical charts for 46 participants, and most of these individuals (n=42) were receiving pharmacological treatment upon admission to the partial hospital (antidepressant: n=34, antianxiety: n=18, mood stabilizer: n=16, antipsychotic: n=16, range=0–7 medications; M=2.70, S.D.=1.50). Correlations among questionnaires and associated subscales are presented in Table 2. Bivariate correlations indicated

Discussion

Attentional control and rumination have received substantial theoretical and empirical interest as transdiagnostic mechanisms underlying psychopathology. While attentional dysfunction and rumination separately appear to serve as transdiagnostic mechanisms in both depression and anxiety, few studies have examined theorized relationships between attentional dysfunction and rumination across depression and anxiety symptoms in a patient population. This study tested the extent to which rumination

References (57)

  • S.J. Kertz et al.

    The importance of worry across diagnostic presentations: prevalence, severity and associated symptoms in a partial hospital setting

    J. Anxiety Disord.

    (2012)
  • E.H.W. Koster et al.

    Understanding depressive rumination from a cognitive science perspective: the impaired disengagement hypothesis

    Clin. Psychol. Rev.

    (2011)
  • K.A. McLaughlin et al.

    Rumination as a transdiagnostic factor in depression and anxiety

    Behav. Res. Ther.

    (2011)
  • T. Mellings et al.

    Cognitive processes in social anxiety: The effects of self-focus, rumination and anticipatory processing

    Behav. Res. Ther.

    (2000)
  • J. Moriya et al.

    Relationships between negative emotionality and attentional control in effortful control

    Personal. Individ. Differ.

    (2008)
  • P. Muris et al.

    Mediating effects of rumination and worry on the links between neuroticism, anxiety and depression

    Personal. Individ. Differ.

    (2005)
  • R.P. Ólafsson et al.

    Self reported attentional control with the attentional control scale: factor structure and relationship with symptoms of anxiety and depression

    J. Anxiety Disord.

    (2011)
  • C. Papageorgiou et al.

    Treatment of recurrent major depression with attention training

    Cogn. Behav. Pract.

    (2000)
  • M.L. Reinholdt-Dunne et al.

    Effects of anxiety and attention control on processing pictorial and linguistic emotional information

    Behav. Res. Ther.

    (2009)
  • J. Roelofs et al.

    Rumination and worrying as possible mediators in the relation between neuroticism and symptoms of depression and anxiety in clinically depressed individuals

    Behav. Res. Ther.

    (2008)
  • K. Surrence et al.

    Brooding and reflective rumination among suicide attempters: cognitive vulnerability to suicidal ideation

    Behav. Res. Ther.

    (2009)
  • L. Wolkenstein et al.

    Cognitive emotion regulation in euthymic bipolar disorder

    J. Affect. Disord.

    (2014)
  • T. Björgvinsson et al.

    Effectiveness of cognitive behavior therapy for severe mood disorders in an acute psychiatric naturalistic setting: a benchmarking study

    Cogn. Behav. Ther.

    (2014)
  • J. Bomyea et al.

    The effect of an executive functioning training program on working memory capacity and intrusive thoughts

    Cogn. Ther. Res.

    (2011)
  • M. Browning et al.

    The modification of attentional bias to emotional information: a review of the techniques, mechanisms, and relevance to emotional disorders

    Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci.

    (2010)
  • R. Chambers et al.

    The impact of intensive mindfulness training on attentional control, cognitive style, and affect

    Cogn. Ther. Res.

    (2008)
  • S. Daches et al.

    Brooding and attentional control in processing self-encoded information: evidence from a modified Garner task

    Cogn. Emot.

    (2010)
  • R. De Raedt et al.

    Understanding vulnerability for depression from a cognitive neuroscience perspective: a reappraisal of attentional factors and a new conceptual framework

    Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci.

    (2010)
  • Cited by (103)

    • Development and plasticity of executive functions: A value-based account

      2022, Current Opinion in Psychology
      Citation Excerpt :

      For instance, mental health problems, such as depression and anxiety, are characterised by increased levels of rumination and worry [47,48]. Poorer attentional control could lead to individuals being more vulnerable to such dysfunctional cognitive styles [48]. Indeed, abnormal attentional control and emotion regulation has been demonstrated to play a significant role in vulnerability to depression and anxiety [47,48].

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text