Elsevier

Intelligence

Volume 52, September–October 2015, Pages 90-96
Intelligence

Smarter every day: The deceleration of population ageing in terms of cognition

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2015.07.005Get rights and content

Highlights

  • We investigate the Flynn effect on several cognitive tests in England and Germany.

  • Individuals aged 50+ perform cognitively better if born into more recent cohorts.

  • The upward drift in mean cognitive abilities is similar across educational levels.

  • The use of modern technology by individuals aged 50+ has increased over time.

  • Trends in modern technology use are associated with the observed Flynn effect.

Abstract

Cognitive decline correlates with age-associated health risks and has been shown to be a good predictor of future morbidity and mortality. Cognitive functioning can therefore be considered an important measure of differential ageing across cohorts and population groups. Here, we investigate if and why individuals aged 50+ born into more recent cohorts perform better in terms of cognition than their counterparts of the same age born into earlier cohorts (Flynn effect). Based on two waves of English and German survey data, we show that cognitive test scores of participants aged 50+ in the later wave are higher compared with those of participants aged 50+ in the earlier wave. The mean scores in the later wave correspond to the mean scores in the earlier wave obtained by participants who were on average 4–8 years younger. The use of a repeat cross-sectional design overcomes potential bias from retest effects. We show for the first time that although compositional changes of the older population in terms of education partly explain the Flynn effect, the increasing use of modern technology (i.e., computers and mobile phones) in the first decade of the 2000s also contributes to its explanation.

Introduction

Higher chronological age tends to be associated with lower cognitive functioning, which in turn correlates with a range of age-associated health risks such as hypertension and diabetes (McCrimmon et al., 2012, Slomski, 2014). Cognitive functioning is also a good predictor of future morbidity and mortality (Negash et al., 2011). Individuals with higher cognitive abilities tend to live longer and healthier lives (Batty et al., 2007, Der et al., 2009). Cognitive functioning can therefore be considered an important measure of differential ageing across cohorts and population groups.

Cognitive functioning is a characteristic of individuals that is associated with but not determined by chronological age. The maintenance of good cognitive functioning is thus one of the central components of successful ageing (Rowe & Kahn, 1987). Some parts of the population start ageing earlier than others, education being a central factor in this regard. Higher educated individuals tend to participate in more cognitively stimulating activities during their lifetime and therefore remain cognitively fit until a higher age (Wilson, Barnes, & Bennett, 2003). Recent evidence shows that the lower educated start ageing earlier than those with higher levels of education — in terms of physical health and fitness (Christensen et al., 2009, Mäki et al., 2013, Sanderson and Scherbov, 2014) as well as in terms of mental health and cognitive functioning (Lièvre, Alley, & Crimmins, 2008). The expansion of education has therefore been among the primary explanations for why steadily increasing average scores on common tests of cognitive functioning (including classic IQ tests) have been observed since the end of the 19th century — the so-called Flynn effect (Flynn, 1984, Flynn, 2000, Flynn, 2007, Hiscock, 2007).

The majority of studies on the Flynn effect have focused on children, adolescents, and prime-age adults. However, some recent studies have shown that the Flynn effect extends to older populations (Baxendale, 2010, Christensen et al., 2013, De Rotrou et al., 2013, Gerstorf et al., 2011, Skirbekk et al., 2013). In other words, older populations today have aged more successfully in terms of their cognitive functioning than did earlier generations. An important reason for such secular trends may lie in the expansion of education and thus the fact that more recent cohorts tend to be more highly educated (Baker et al., 2015, Ceci, 1991).

The factors responsible for the Flynn effect are commonly assumed to be environmental rather than genetic given the speed at which average cognitive abilities have been rising (Dickens and Flynn, 2001, Flynn, 2007, Pietschnig and Voracek, 2015). In addition to improved education (Baker et al., 2015, Teasdale and Owen, 2005), a range of environmental explanations for the Flynn effect have been proposed such as improvements in medical care and nutrition (Lynn, 2009), reduced family size (Sundet et al., 2008, Zajonc and Mullally, 1997), and reductions in pathogen stress (Eppig, Fincher, & Thornhill, 2010). These formerly independent theories for explaining the Flynn effect (i.e., better education, improved nutrition, reduced family size, and lower pathogen stress) have been integrated by Woodley in a model of life history speed (Woodley, 2012a) that points to the important insight that the explanations for the Flynn effect are likely to vary across space and time.

In the context of explanations based on changes in environmental conditions, Neisser (1997) put forward the thesis that the increasing exposure to visual and technical environments may be a central factor for explaining the Flynn effect. The increasing use of computers and gadgets with visual interfaces in everyday life may have contributed to a rising complexity of our cognitive environments and in turn higher levels of cognitive stimulation and performance (Charness et al., 2011, Dickens and Flynn, 2001, Neisser, 1997, Schooler, 1998). Especially the older parts of the population who used to withdraw from cognitively demanding tasks and environments at relatively young ages (i.e., mental retirement, cf. Rohwedder & Willis, 2010) are increasingly exposed to cognitive challenges in everyday life (Charness & Schaie, 2003). The thesis that the increasing use of modern technology in everyday life increases cognitive demands on the older population and in turn helps maintain cognitive capacities to higher ages is put to an empirical test in the present study.

This study aims to contribute to our understanding of why individuals at age 50 or above today perform better in tests of cognitive functioning than people of the same age did in the past. First, to ascertain the presence of a Flynn effect, we investigate change over time in the cognitive functioning of the population aged 50+ in England and Germany. We use a wide range of different cognitive tests that differ in their sensitiveness to the biological ageing processes of the brain (Hiscock, 2007), including tests of processing speed and accuracy, a test of verbal fluency, and two tests of verbal memory. Second, we test if education and the increasing use of modern technology help explain the Flynn effect. To date, evidence for the use of modern technology as a cause for the Flynn effect is still warranted (Pietschnig & Voracek, 2015).

Studies pointing to a sustained upward drift in mean cognitive abilities over time (i.e., the presence of a Flynn effect) have usually paid little attention to subgroup differences by age, sex, or level of education (Ang, Rodgers, & Wänström, 2010). Recent exceptions include the work by Pietschnig and Voracek (2015) who find significantly stronger Flynn effects for adults than for children and adolescents. Ang et al. (2010) find an accelerated Flynn effect for children of more educated mothers. No sex differences in the Flynn effect have been found (Pietschnig, Voracek, & Formann, 2011). The present study explores age and sex differences in the Flynn effect as well as differences by educational attainment.

Section snippets

Surveys and sampling

This study is based on data from two surveys, the German Socio-Economic Panel (SOEP) and the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing (ELSA). In order to avoid retest effects, we do not use the longitudinal samples of these surveys but restrict the samples of analysis to individuals taking part in the cognitive tests only once.

SOEP is a survey of private households that provides representative, longitudinal micro-data that have been collected on an annual basis since 1984. All samples are

Descriptive findings

The summary statistics shown in Table 1a, Table 1b suggest that average cognitive functioning is higher in the more recent survey wave as compared with the earlier one both in England and in Germany (i.e., confirming the existence of a Flynn effect). The effect sizes are similar across all measures with a Cohen's d (Cohen, 1988) of about 0.2 standard deviations (0.21 for SDT90 and DR, 0.20 for ANT, 0.17 for IR and LCT, 0.15 for SDT60), except for STD30 (0.09 SD). Based on an average IQ test

Discussion

This paper contributed to the discussion of the sustained upward drift in mean cognitive abilities over time, adding an analysis of the role played by the use of modern information and communication technology for maintaining cognitive functioning in the older population. We presented a simple procedure that uses measures of cognitive functioning as a characteristic-based age (Sanderson & Scherbov, 2013) and expresses the magnitude of the Flynn effect in terms of a secular ‘age-gain’ — a

Contributors

All authors contributed equally to the work. All three authors contributed to the overall study design. V.B. performed the analyses on ELSA data and N.S. the analyses on SOEP data. S.S. directed the study. N.S. and V.B. wrote the manuscript, which was edited by all the co-authors. All authors have approved the final article.

Acknowledgments

The authors acknowledge the funding from the European Research Council Grant no ERC2013-AdG 323947-Re-Ageing. The funder was not involved in the research.

The data on England were made available through the UK Data Archive. ELSA was developed by a team of researchers based at the University College of London, the National Centre for Social Research and the Institute for Fiscal Studies. The data were collected by the National Centre for Social Research. The developers and funders of ELSA and the

References (63)

  • F. Mazzonna et al.

    Ageing, cognitive abilities and retirement

    European Economic Review

    (2012)
  • R.J. McCrimmon et al.

    Diabetes and cognitive dysfunction

    Lancet

    (2012)
  • S. Negash et al.

    Successful aging: Definitions and prediction of longevity and conversion to mild cognitive impairment

    The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry

    (2011)
  • J. Pietschnig et al.

    Female Flynn effects: No sex differences in generational IQ gains

    Personality and Individual Differences

    (2011)
  • M. Rönnlund et al.

    The magnitude, generality, and determinants of Flynn effects on forms of declarative memory and visuospatial ability: Time-sequential analyses of data from a Swedish cohort study

    Intelligence

    (2008)
  • V. Skirbekk et al.

    The Flynn effect and population aging

    Intelligence

    (2013)
  • J.M. Sundet et al.

    The end of the Flynn effect?: A study of secular trends in mean intelligence test scores of Norwegian conscripts during half a century

    Intelligence

    (2004)
  • J.M. Sundet et al.

    The Flynn effect is partly caused by changing fertility patterns

    Intelligence

    (2008)
  • T.W. Teasdale et al.

    A long-term rise and recent decline in intelligence test performance: The Flynn Effect in reverse

    Personality and Individual Differences

    (2005)
  • M.A. Woodley

    A life history model of the Lynn–Flynn effect

    Personality and Individual Differences

    (2012)
  • M.A. Woodley

    The social and scientific temporal correlates of genotypic intelligence and the Flynn effect

    Intelligence

    (2012)
  • J.A. Anguera et al.

    Video game training enhances cognitive control in older adults

    Nature

    (2013)
  • J. Banks et al.

    The effect of education on old age cognitive abilities: Evidence from a regression discontinuity design

    The Economic Journal

    (2012)
  • S. Baxendale

    The Flynn effect and memory function

    Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology

    (2010)
  • R. Cattell

    Intelligence: Its structure, growth and action

    (1987)
  • S.J. Ceci

    How much does schooling influence general intelligence and its cognitive components? A reassessment of the evidence

    Developmental Psychology

    (1991)
  • N. Charness et al.

    Life-span cognition and information technology

  • N. Charness et al.

    The impact of technology on successful aging

    (2003)
  • H. Cheshire et al.

    Financial circumstances, health and wellbeing of the older population in England

    The 2008 English Longitudinal Study of Ageing: Technical report

    (2012)
  • J. Cohen

    Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences

    (1988)
  • Cited by (23)

    • The Flynn effect in Europe – Effects of sex and region

      2017, Intelligence
      Citation Excerpt :

      Furthermore, modern findings of gender parity in IQ assessments, combined with some historical accounts of a female deficit relative to men, suggest that women should have been gaining faster than men (Flynn, 2012). Earlier studies examining sex differences in the Flynn effect are few and most have reported similar Flynn effects for men and women (Ang, Rodgers, & Wänström, 2010; Bordone, Scherbov, & Steiber, 2015; Pietschnig, Voracek, & Formann, 2011; Rönnlund & Nilsson, 2008). These findings, however, have been based on small samples and study populations very distinct from ours (Ang et al., 2010; Pietschnig et al., 2011).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text