Elsevier

Intelligence

Volume 41, Issue 5, September–October 2013, Pages 537-545
Intelligence

An account of the relationship between fluid intelligence and complex learning in considering storage capacity and executive attention

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2013.07.008Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Clarification of the nature of the overlap of Gf and complex learning (CL).

  • Storage capacity and executive attention were proposed to account for the overlap.

  • Both storage capacity and executive attention contributed to Gf and CL.

  • Storage capacity accounted for most of the shared variance between Gf and CL.

Abstract

Although fluid intelligence and complex learning are conceptualized differently and assessed by apparently different measures, both theoretical accounts and empirical evidence suggest a relationship between the two constructs. In this study, major working memory aspects including the storage capacity and executive attention were proposed to account for the relationship between fluid intelligence and complex learning. A sample of 184 participants completed fluid intelligence and complex learning scales, as well as working memory measures that each included two or three treatment levels differing in the demands on capacity or executive control. The differences among the treatment levels provided a favorable precondition for employing fixed-links models to separate the core processes of storage capacity or executive attention from the auxiliary processes. Results indicated that both storage capacity and executive attention contributed significantly to fluid intelligence and complex learning. A further analysis showed that the two working memory aspects, particularly the storage capacity, accounted for most of the shared variance between fluid intelligence and complex learning.

Introduction

Fluid intelligence reflects the ability to reason and to solve problems in novel situations where prior experience and acquired knowledge are of no help (Horn & Cattell, 1967) while learning mainly refers to the acquisition of new information permanently or the modification of previously stored knowledge (Sweller, 2005). Fluid intelligence and learning originated from different psychological traditions. Previous research on fluid intelligence and learning so far has been conducted as part of two distinct disciplines of scientific psychology (Cronbach, 1957): the study of intelligence mainly occurred in differential psychology with the purpose of revealing sources of the variation underlying human abilities whereas the study of learning was mainly conducted in experimental psychology aiming at discovering the general laws of behavior without reference to individual variation (Jensen, 1989). In spite of this, available evidence suggests a relationship between fluid intelligence and learning, especially learning in complex situations (e.g., Alexander and Smales, 1997, Williams et al., 2008, Williams and Pearlberg, 2006). This study is to explore the nature of the relationship between fluid intelligence and complex learning in considering two major aspects of working memory, i.e., the storage capacity and executive attention, as suspected cognitive source of the relationship.

Human intelligence and learning are commonly assumed to be related with each other since the early conceptualization of intelligence which seems to be strongly tied to learning ability (e.g., Buckingham, 1921, p. 273). Furthermore, theoretical developments in different research traditions have highlighted this relationship. For example, in the framework of individual differences in skill learning, Ackerman, 1988, Ackerman, 2007 concluded that human abilities, especially general intelligence, play a substantial part in determining individual differences in task performance at the early stage of learning procedures. There is also the investment theory stating that fluid intelligence is invested in the development of crystallized abilities (Cattell, 1963). A revision of the investment theory even suggests that learning processes may serve as the driving factor in accounting for the role of fluid intelligence in acquiring developed abilities and specific areas of knowledge (Schweizer & Koch, 2002).

Despite those theories associating learning with fluid intelligence, empirical studies do not suggest that all sorts of learning are equally associated with fluid intelligence. It appears that learning is mostly related to fluid intelligence when the learning material is of at least moderate difficulty and complexity (Lohman, 1999). For example, Woodrow, 1938, Woodrow, 1946 has reported rather weak or no correlations between performance in a variety of learning tasks and intelligence scores. A general critique of the Woodrow's studies is that the learning tasks he employed were simple and unlikely to be related to complex cognitive abilities (Estes, 1970). Recent research conducted by Williams and Pearlberg (2006) shows that fluid intelligence is more strongly related to the learning rate of complex learning tasks than to that of simple learning tasks (see also Kaufman et al., 2009, Tamez et al., 2008). Therefore, in investigating individual differences of learning and related abilities, it is necessary to make explicit the type of learning that is under consideration. In this study, we concentrate on complex learning, which involves the acquisition and development of a series of goal-directed strategies and abstract rules (cf. Anderson, Fincham, & Douglass, 1997).

As indicated by previous research, complex learning and fluid intelligence show an established relationship, which indicates an overlap between the two constructs. A question of interest is where the overlap comes from or what the nature of the overlap is. To answer this question, it is of great significance to consider the cognitive sources that are crucial for both fluid intelligence and complex learning. One of the most promising candidates in individual differences research is working memory.

Working memory is a limited-capacity system allowing the simultaneous storage and manipulation of information during completing complex tasks such as reasoning, learning and comprehension (Baddeley, 1986). In an influential model proposed by Baddeley and Hitch (1974), working memory includes two domain-specific storage components responsible for storing verbal and visuospatial information, and a domain-general component, the central executive system that is responsible for the allocation of attentional resources for maintaining or storing information and processing dual tasks.

The relation between working memory and fluid intelligence has been repeatedly investigated and working memory has been shown as a powerful predictor of fluid intelligence (Colom et al., 2008, Conway et al., 2002, Engle et al., 1999, Kane et al., 2005, Kane et al., 2004). In particular, a few of those studies have demonstrated that working memory capacity and fluid intelligence share from around 50% to 70% of their variances (e.g., Kane et al., 2005, Oberauer et al., 2005). Moreover, researchers in this field have already tried to explain why working memory is strongly related to fluid intelligence. Two aspects or specific properties of working memory have been put forth to account for the predictive power of working memory. One group of theories suggests that the capacity to maintain a distinct number of separate representations active for ongoing processing is the genuine contributor to fluid intelligence. A number of studies indicated that this storage capacity of working memory accounts for a unique variance of fluid intelligence independent of other components of working memory (Chuderski et al., 2012, Colom et al., 2008, Cowan et al., 2006, Unsworth and Engle, 2007).

A second group of theories suggests that the predictive power of working memory derives from the quality of attention control or executive attention, which is typically involved in monitoring ongoing procedures, the selective activation of relevant representations, and the suppression of irrelevant or distracting ones. This seems in line with Baddeley's (1986) emphasis of the central executive as the primary underlying construct in working memory system. There are also quite a few studies demonstrating that performance in both working memory and fluid intelligence measures depends on the quality of the domain-general control of attention (henceforth called executive attention) (e.g., Engle et al., 1999, Kane et al., 2007, Kane et al., 2004, Ren et al., 2013, Unsworth and Spillers, 2010).

Theoretical developments on learning and skill acquisition have also emphasized the importance of working memory during complex learning (Anderson et al., 1997, Paas et al., 2010, Sweller, 1988, Sweller, 2005, Wittrock, 1992). For instance, in the four-stage model of skill acquisition proposed by Anderson et al. (1997), individuals firstly have to keep specific examples or items in working memory for developing the abstract schema or rules. Secondly, the rules that are derived from those examples should also be maintained in working memory for further using before they are transferred into long-term memory. Another theoretical account that associates working memory with complex learning is the cognitive load theory, which highlights the influence of the limited storage capacity of working memory on learning processes (Sweller, 1988, Sweller, 2005). Since in complex learning activities learning schemas can be formed only when the amount of information that has to be processed does not exceed the processing capacity of working memory, the capacity of working memory is becoming particularly critical. Moreover, learning performance in complex domains usually requires the acquisition of a great number of schemas, individuals with larger working memory capacity are assumed to construct more appropriate schemas to facilitate learning processes.

Although fluid intelligence and complex learning are conceptualized differently and assessed by apparently different measures, both theoretical accounts and empirical evidence suggest a relationship between the two constructs. In the present study, working memory is proposed to account for the overlap between fluid intelligence and complex learning. In particular, since working memory is not a unitary construct characterized by a sole aspect of information processing, we concentrate on the two major aspects of working memory, the storage capacity and executive attention, both of which have been suggested to be involved in intelligence and learning performance (e.g., Anderson et al., 1997, Kane et al., 2007, Ren et al., 2013, Sweller, 1988, Sweller, 2005, Unsworth and Engle, 2007, Unsworth and Spillers, 2010). We investigate whether the variance shared by fluid intelligence and complex learning can be explained by working memory storage capacity and executive attention. If so, a further goal is to examine if each of the two factors accounts for a unique portion of the overlap or whether one factor accounts for the overlap only because it covaries with the second factor which serves as the genuine contributor.

To better represent the storage capacity and executive attention, we employ and modify established working memory measures. The aim of the modification is to manipulate the demands associated with a particular property of working memory. Because of the experimental manipulation, each measure is enlarged to have two or three treatment levels instead of one. Furthermore, a special kind of confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) model, i.e., the fixed-links model, is applied to analyze those experimental data so that the underlying processes assessed by a cognitive measure can be decomposed into two components: the core processes of storage capacity or executive attention, and the auxiliary processes that reflect basic processing speed or motor processes (cf. Schweizer, 2007). Different from the conventional structural equation modeling (SEM) in which links between manifest and latent variables are freely estimated, the factor loadings in the fixed-links models are fixed to a series of values in order to reflect experimental manipulations.

Section snippets

Participants

The sample consisted of 184 university students (63 males and 121 females) aged between 18 and 40 years (M = 23.85, SD = 5.05). Most of the participants received financial rewards for participating. A few of them got course credit.

Exchange Test (ET)

This test was employed to assess the storage capacity of working memory (Schweizer, 1996, Schweizer, 2007). Participants were asked to mentally reorder the serial positions of symbols in completing this test. In each item two arrays of the same four symbols were presented

Descriptive results

Table 1 presents the descriptive statistics for each treatment level of ET and SCT, each set of CLT and the intelligence scales. The mean accuracy scores of the three treatment levels of ET decreased from the first to the third levels. A repeated-measure analysis of variance indicated significant differences between the three treatment levels, F (2, 366) = 196.06, p < .001, partial η2 = 0.52. The mean accuracy score of the second treatment level of SCT was lower than that of the first treatment

Discussion

Individual differences in learning and intelligence seem to describe different aspects of human ability: the former focuses more on the process of acquiring and developing knowledge-based skills and strategies while the latter tends to focus on reasoning or problem solving. In spite of that, theory and research related to the topic of the present paper suggest that learning and intelligence are not essentially independent sources of individual differences (e.g., Alexander and Smales, 1997,

References (48)

  • X. Ren et al.

    The sources of the relationship between sustained attention and reasoning

    Intelligence

    (2013)
  • K. Schweizer

    The speed–accuracy transition due to task complexity

    Intelligence

    (1996)
  • K. Schweizer

    Investigating the relationship of working memory tasks and fluid intelligence tests by means of the fixed-links model in considering the impurity problem

    Intelligence

    (2007)
  • K. Schweizer et al.

    A revision of Cattell's Investment Theory: Cognitive properties influencing learning

    Learning and Individual Differences

    (2002)
  • K. Schweizer et al.

    The structure of the relationship between attention and intelligence

    Intelligence

    (2005)
  • J. Sweller

    Cognitive load during problem solving: Effects on learning

    Cognitive Science

    (1988)
  • E. Tamez et al.

    Learning, working memory, and intelligence revisited

    Behavioural Processes

    (2008)
  • N. Unsworth et al.

    Working memory capacity: Attention, memory, or both? A direct test of the dual-component model

    Journal of Memory and Language

    (2010)
  • B.A. Williams et al.

    Learning of three-term contingencies correlates with Raven scores, but not with measures of cognitive processing

    Intelligence

    (2006)
  • P.L. Ackerman

    Determinants of individual differences during skill acquisition: Cognitive abilities and information processing

    Journal of Experimental Psychology. General

    (1988)
  • P.L. Ackerman

    New developments in understanding skilled performance

    Current Directions in Psychological Science

    (2007)
  • J.R. Anderson et al.

    The role of examples and rules in the acquisition of a cognitive skill

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition

    (1997)
  • A.D. Baddeley

    Working memory

    (1986)
  • A.D. Baddeley et al.

    Working memory

  • Cited by (16)

    • Attention in the heart of intelligence

      2018, Trends in Neuroscience and Education
      Citation Excerpt :

      Also, studies with children and adults have shown that both the scope and control of attention contribute to individual differences in intelligence, and that the internal control of attention acts in the service of memory processes to influence reasoning skills [12]. Hence, there is evidence that both storage capacity and executive attention contribute significantly to fluid intelligence and complex learning skills [14] both in adulthood and during development. The three general functions of attention just described have been associated with distinct brain networks within Posner's neurocognitive model of attention [15,2].

    • Comparing the effectiveness of different methods to reduce the effect size of the practice effect in traffic psychological assessment

      2018, Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour
      Citation Excerpt :

      The extent to which repeated test-takers will be advantaged depends on the effect size of the practice effect, which in turn depends on the extent to which specific cognitive abilities can be modified by means of repeated practice. Furthermore, since strategy improvements are gradual and depend on freely available working memory capacity (cf. Ren, Goldhammer, Moosbrugger, & Schweizer, 2012; Ropovik, 2014; Wang, Ren, Altmeyer, & Schweizer, 2013) any means that reduce the available working memory resources during test-taking should reduce the size of the practice effect by reducing the chance to learn during test-taking and/or reducing the likelihood that newly acquired, more effective and efficient processing strategies are used during the retest session. As a consequence repeated test-takers would be less advantaged over first-time test-takers with an equal level of broad cognitive speededness (Gs).

    • On the relationship between executive functions of working memory and components derived from fluid intelligence measures

      2017, Acta Psychologica
      Citation Excerpt :

      The same argument applies to shifting since the kinds of cognitive operations to be performed are usually quite restricted so that there is frequent shifting between similar cognitive operations. Furthermore, because of individual differences in learning in combination with individual differences in the capacity of working memory (Wang, Ren, Altmeyer, & Schweizer, 2013), improvements in the efficiency of the updating and shifting processes are likely to occur in the course of completing items of an intelligence test. Such a learning interpretation of the item-position effect provides a further argument for the relationship between executive functioning and intelligence.

    • Beyond the intellect: Complexity and learning trajectories in Raven's Progressive Matrices depend on self-regulatory processes and conative dispositions

      2017, Intelligence
      Citation Excerpt :

      The final difference is in how we attempt to understand the latent factors. Schweizer and his colleagues (e.g., Ren et al., 2012; Schweizer, 2006; Schweizer et al., 2015; Wang et al., 2013) have explored fixed-links models to partition variance in numerous cognitive functions (e.g., attention, WM, and learning), whereas we have simultaneously explored general reasoning ability (as a covariate) and moderating factors in the personality realm, while also introducing an experimental catalyst to prime self-reflection, which in practice seems to have accentuated associations. It is interesting to note, that in our data, item-order effects did not emerge alone as in Schweizer's work, but were only present in association with personality moderators.

    • Does the modality of measures influence the relationship among working memory, learning and fluid intelligence?

      2017, Personality and Individual Differences
      Citation Excerpt :

      However, the empirical findings regarding the correlations between performance on learning and fluid intelligence measures are inconsistent. For instance, Woodrow (1938, 1946) found rather weak or no correlations between performance in a variety of learning tasks and intelligence scores, whereas more recent studies reveal significant relationships of fluid intelligence with associative learning (Kaufman, DeYoung, Gray, Brown, & Mackintosh, 2009; Tamez, Myerson, & Hale, 2012; Williams & Pearlberg, 2006) and complex learning (Ren, Wang, Altmeyer, & Schweizer, 2014; Wang et al., 2013). The inconsistency regarding the relationship between fluid intelligence and learning may partly be due to the differences between the modalities to which the measures refer.

    View all citing articles on Scopus

    This research was supported by a grant of the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft, Kennedyallee 40, 53175 Bonn, Germany (SCHW 402/14-1).

    View full text