Elsevier

Intelligence

Volume 40, Issue 2, March–April 2012, Pages 151-162
Intelligence

Sex differences in mental rotation and cortical activation patterns: Can training change them?

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2012.01.005Get rights and content

Abstract

In two experiments the neuronal mechanisms of sex differences in mental rotation were investigated. In Experiment 1 cortical activation was studied in women and men with similar levels of mental rotation ability (high, and average to low), who were equalized with respect to general intelligence. Sex difference in neuroelectric patterns of brain activity were observed only in participants with high mental rotation ability. Females displayed more theta synchronization, especially in frontal brain areas. In the second experiment we examined whether training can increase mental rotation performance in females and change their brain activity patterns measured with neuroelectric and hemodynamic imaging techniques. In a parallel group experimental design, respondents from the origami group (rotation training), after 18 h of training, significantly increased their performance on a test of mental rotation. Females' brain activation patterns on a posttest, as compared with a pretest, showed decreased frontal brain activity. Parallel to this, increased activity in parietal brain areas was observed. By contrast, respondents from the active control group (participating in 18-hour communication training) showed no improvements in performance and no pre-/posttest differences in cortical activity.

Highlights

► The neuronal mechanisms of sex differences in mental rotation were investigated. ► Women high in mental rotation ability displayed more theta synchronization in frontal brain areas. ► Origami training increased females' performance on a mental rotation test. ► After training cortical activity of females was similar to males high in mental rotation ability.

Introduction

Men typically demonstrate distinct advantages on a broad range of visual tasks. The most pronounced sex differences of nearly one standard deviation have been reported mainly for mental rotation, the ability to imagine visual shapes rotated to an orientation other than the one in which they are presented (Mackintosh and Bennett, 2005, Masters and Sanders, 1993, Voyer et al., 1995). Mental rotation tasks can therefore serve as a useful probe for investigating the neural underpinnings of sex differences in cognition. Studies investigating this phenomenon have produced rather diverse results.

Several functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies have reported significant differences in brain activity between males and females who were involved in solving mental rotation tasks, even though no differences in performance were observed (Butler et al., 2006, Hugdahl et al., 2006, Jordan et al., 2002). On the other hand, some studies have reported no significant between-sex differences in brain activity, although men outperformed women on the spatial rotational tasks used during brain imaging (Bell et al., 2006, Halari et al., 2006). Furthermore, there are still other studies – employing neuroelectric or hemodynamic brain imaging techniques – showing sex differences in performance as well as brain activation patterns (Gootjes et al., 2008a, Gootjes et al., 2008b, Gur et al., 2000, Hahn et al., 2009).

It has been observed that, while solving mental rotation tasks, males showed predominantly parietal activation, whereas females also showed frontal brain activity (Butler et al., 2006, Hugdahl et al., 2006). A meta-analysis of some early studies on sex differences in brain lateralization in relation to mental rotation have indicated a strong preference for the right hemisphere in males, while females showed no hemispheric advantage (Vogel, Bowers, & Vogel, 2003). On the other hand, some recent studies have revealed that, during spatial processing, men showed greater and more bilateral activation than women (Gur et al., 2000), and that such differences can even be observed in preschool children (Hahn et al., 2009).

Despite some specific inconsistencies in the findings, it has most often been suggested that men and women apply different strategies while solving mental rotation tasks (Butler et al., 2006, Gootjes et al., 2008a, Gootjes et al., 2008b, Hugdahl et al., 2006, Jordan et al., 2002). It was hypothesized that women performed mental rotation tasks in an effortful, “top-down” way, whereas men's performances used an automatic, “bottom-up” strategy. The top-down processing was linked to the activation of the prefrontal cortex, associated with decision making and spatial working memory. In contrast, the bottom-up approach was associated with less prefrontal involvement, less frontal control of task performance and a greater involvement of primary sensory brain regions (Butler et al., 2006). A similar distinction was proposed by Hugdahl et al. (2006), who associated the frontal lobe activity of females with a serial reasoning strategy and activity in the parietal lobe with a “gestalt” perceptual strategy displayed by men. On the other hand, Gootjes et al., 2008a, Gootjes et al., 2008b related male right hemispheric activity with a holistic strategy, and a more intense left hemispheric activity observed in females with an analytic strategy.

Some authors have further suggested that these differences in brain activation patterns are the result of sex-specific organization of the neural networks involved in spatial processing (Jordan et al., 2002) and are present in children as young as 5 (Hahn et al., 2009). In contrast, other authors have suggested that the differences are the result of experience and the differential participation of male and female children in activities, combined with hormonal changes during puberty (Jordan and Wüstenberg, 2010, Roberts and Bell, 2002). Support for the latter hypothesis is provided by recent studies showing that mental rotation performance can be enhanced with practice, and that these enhancements are sex-specific (Feng et al., 2007, Neidhardt and Popp, 2010, Quaiser-Pohl and Lehmann, 2002).

There are three main shortcomings that might have biased the differences in brain activity between males and females as reported in the studies reviewed:

First, the participants were not matched for their level of general cognitive ability or intelligence although there exist numerous findings establishing the intelligence–brain relationship. Even though there is no firm consensus about any particular brain–intelligence relation, most of the studies demonstrated a negative correlation between brain activity under cognitive load and intelligence (for review, Neubauer & Fink, 2009). The explanation for these findings was an efficiency theory: the non-use of many brain areas irrelevant to the task performance, as well as the more focused use of specific task-relevant areas in individuals with high IQ (Haier, 1993). This inverse intelligence–activation relationship appears to be moderated by task content, the individual's sex, and motivation — when individuals estimate that they cannot solve a problem, they stop trying. (Doppelmayr et al., 2005, Jaušovec and Jaušovec, 2008, Neubauer and Fink, 2003, Neubauer et al., 2002, Neubauer et al., 2005).

Second, respondents were not matched with respect to spatial ability, which is an important component of general intelligence (Kaufman, 2007). The importance of spatial ability and especially of mental rotation has been argued quite recently by Johnson and Bouchard (2005). They posit that the fluid and crystallized intelligence stratum should be replaced with a stratum consisting of verbal, perceptual, and image rotation ability. Johnson and Bouchard (2007) further found that sex differences on these dimensions were strong. Both men and women were located throughout the ranges of dimensions, but women were concentrated towards the verbal poles, and men were concentrated towards the rotation poles. That the rotation–verbal dimension was related to brain structure was demonstrated in a recent study by Johnson, Jung, Colom, & Haier (2008), who correlated the rotation–verbal dimension with brain volumes of gray and white matter. The correlations involving the rotation–verbal dimension were relatively localized to areas of the brain that have been implicated in tasks involving the relevant content areas in prior studies. As argued by Johnson and Bouchard (2007) the relation between general cognitive ability and specific abilities, like mental rotation, contribute to the difficulties we experience in trying to identify the brain functions underpinning sex differences in mental abilities. For example, a female of high intelligence but little mental rotation ability who is solving a mental rotation task may do as well as a male of lower intelligence who has greater mental rotation ability. This could be so because the two people would use different strategies and brain areas.

The third factor that could have biased the reported differences in brain activity between males and females is the relationship between hormonal changes during the female's menstrual cycle, performance on spatial rotation tasks and brain activity. It has been shown that the relative release of sexual hormones in different phases of the menstrual cycle affects cognitive and affective responses of females. High levels of estrogen and progesterone have been associated with positive affect and higher activity in the prefrontal cortex (Amin et al., 2006, Berman et al., 1997). Some studies have reported that women perform better on mental rotation tasks during cycle phases characterized by low, rather than high estrogen levels (Hampson, 1990, Phillips and Silverman, 1997). Moreover, it has been shown that blood estrogen level had a profound effect on the size but not on the lateralization or the localization of cortical activation patterns (Dietrich et al., 2001).

The objective of the present study was to further investigate the neuronal mechanisms of sex differences in mental rotation. For that purpose two experiments were conducted.

Section snippets

Experiment 1

Cortical activation was studied in women and men with similar levels of mental rotation ability (high, and average to low), who were equalized with respect to general intelligence. Additionally, all women were tested during the low-estrogen phase of the menstrual cycle. Such an experimental design allowed us to evaluate the factor of sex, independent of differences in ability levels and hormonal changes.

Brain activity was assessed with an electroencephalogram (EEG). Event-related

Experiment 2

In the second experiment, we examined whether training could increase mental rotation performance in a sample of females with a normal distribution of general intelligence and mental rotation ability — resembling distributions in larger samples where sex differences of nearly one standard deviation in mental rotation performance have been observed (Voyer et al., 1995).

A second objective was to discover whether sex-differences in brain activity patterns observed during mental rotation could be

General discussion

The objective of the present study was to investigate sex difference in performance and brain activity related to the ability of mental rotation. In Experiment 1, an experimental design was used that allowed us to evaluate the factor sex in relation to mental rotation, independent of differences in ability levels and hormonal changes in females. The main finding was that differences in cortical activation patterns between men and women were observed only in participants of an above average

References (66)

  • E. Hampson

    Variations in sex-related cognitive abilities across the menstrual cycle

    Brain and Cognition

    (1990)
  • K. Hugdahl et al.

    Sex differences in visuo-spatial processing: An fMRI study of mental rotation

    Neuropsychologia

    (2006)
  • N. Jaušovec et al.

    Spatial-rotation and recognizing emotions: Gender related differences in brain activity

    Intelligence

    (2008)
  • N. Jaušovec et al.

    The influence of Mozart's music on brain activity in the process of learning

    Clinical Neurophysioogyl

    (2006)
  • W. Johnson et al.

    The structure of human intelligence: It's verbal, perceptual, and image rotation (VPR), not fluid and crystallized

    Intelligence

    (2005)
  • W. Johnson et al.

    Sex differences in mental ability: G masks the dimensions on which they lie

    Intelligence

    (2007)
  • W. Johnson et al.

    Cognitive abilities independent of IQ correlate with regional brain structure

    Intelligence

    (2008)
  • K. Jordan et al.

    Women and men exhibit different cortical activation patterns during mental rotation tasks

    Neuropsychologia

    (2002)
  • S.B. Kaufman

    Sex differences in mental rotation and spatial visualization ability: Can they be accounted for by differences in working memory capacity?

    Intelligence

    (2007)
  • W. Klimesch

    EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: A review and analysis

    Brain Research Review

    (1999)
  • W. Klimesch et al.

    A short review of slow phase synchronization and memory: Evidence for control processes in different memory systems?

    Brain Research

    (2008)
  • S.E. Kober et al.

    Sex differences in human EEG theta oscillations during spatial navigation in virtual reality

    International Journal of Psychophysiology

    (2011)
  • A.C. Neubauer et al.

    Two- vs. three-dimensional presentation of mental rotation tasks: Sex differences and effects of training on performance and brain activation

    Intelligence

    (2010)
  • A.C. Neubauer et al.

    Fluid intelligence and neural efficiency: Effects of task complexity and sex

    Personality and Individual Differences

    (2003)
  • A.C. Neubauer et al.

    Intelligence and neural efficiency

    Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews

    (2009)
  • A.C. Neubauer et al.

    Intelligence and neural efficiency: The influence of task content and sex on the brain–IQ relationship

    Intelligence

    (2002)
  • A.C. Neubauer et al.

    Intelligence and neural efficiency: Further evidence of the influence of task content and sex on the brain–IQ relationship

    Cognitive Brain Research

    (2005)
  • A.C. Neubauer et al.

    Intelligence and individual differences in becoming neurally efficient

    Acta Psychologica

    (2004)
  • I.R. Olson et al.

    Some surprising findings on the involvement of the parietal lobe in human memory

    Neurobiology of Learning and Memory

    (2009)
  • K. Phillips et al.

    Differences in the relationship of menstrual cycle phase to spatial performance on two- and three-dimensional tasks

    Hormonal Behaviour

    (1997)
  • P. Sauseng et al.

    Control mechanisms in working memory: A possible function of EEG theta oscillations

    Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews

    (2010)
  • J.J. Vogel et al.

    Cerebral lateralization of spatial abilities: A meta-analysis

    Brain and Cognition

    (2003)
  • C. Andrev

    Quantification of event-related coherence (ERCoh)

  • Cited by (0)

    View full text