Elsevier

Intelligence

Volume 32, Issue 3, May–June 2004, Pages 245-262
Intelligence

Dimensions of cognition in infancy

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2004.01.004Get rights and content

Abstract

The present study explored the dimensionality of infant cognition by factor analyzing measures from a battery of tasks administered to a large cohort of 7-month-old preterm and full-term infants (N=203). A principal axis factor analysis yielded three factors accounting for 37% of the variance. There was one attention factor (look duration and shift rate), one speed factor (reaction time), and one memory factor (immediate recognition). The results indicate that infant cognition is better characterized as multidimensional than as unitary, and support the idea that many of the tasks used in infancy tap discrete elementary abilities.

Introduction

Information has been accruing about the existence and persistence of individual differences in infant cognition, especially for measures of information processing. Significant reliability and/or stability has been reported for a number of infant measures including attention, processing speed, memory, and those capturing representational competence (such as object permanence, symbolic play, cross-modal transfer, categorization—for an overview, see Rose & Tamis-Lemonda, 1999). These information processing measures all show strong age-related change, differentiate risk groups from their age-matched nonrisk peers, and predict later cognition. Literature reviews indicate that habituation and visual recognition memory, two of the most widely assessed infant abilities, predict later IQ with a median correlation of r=.45 (McCall & Carriger, 1993). This level of prediction, although modest, is better for information processing measures than for traditional psychometric infant tests, which generally have little predictive validity before 2 years for either normal or risk groups Kopp & McCall, 1982, McCall et al., 1972. A critical question arises as to whether these newer measures are predictive because they tap a single underlying ability or, rather, as is more likely, they tap discrete abilities that are among the building blocks of later cognition (Rose & Tamis-Lemonda, 1999).

In trying to understand the basis for these predictive relations, questions about the dimensionality (i.e., factor structure) of infant cognitive abilities arise. While questions about dimensional structure play a large role in studies of adult cognition Cattell, 1971, Guilford, 1967, Horn, 1968, Spearman, 1923, Sternberg, 1985, this issue has been explored very little in infancy. In fact, only three studies have even touched on the problem. In one of these studies (Rose & Feldman, 1995), where a canonical correlation was used to assess the relation between several different measures of information processing in infancy and at 11 years, a single canonical variate emerged. Given the pattern of loadings at both ages, and the fact that many of the 11-year measures were timed tests, the variate was interpreted as reflecting processing speed. In the second study (Rose & Feldman, 1997), where path analysis was used to relate infant visual recognition memory to later IQ, memory, as well as speed, played a role, suggesting that there is more than a single dimension to infant cognition. In the third study (Jacobson et al., 1992), the single-factor analytic study of infant cognition, four factors explained 60.6% of the variance among 13 measures obtained between 6.5 and 13 months. Speed and memory emerged as two of the factors, along with developmental level and one uninterpretable factor.

The present study is a further exploration of the dimensionality of infant cognition. Here, the measures were drawn from four domains: memory, processing speed, attention, and representational competence. These domains not only figure prominently in models of adult human cognition, but in infant cognition as well. Indeed, it has been suggested that individual differences in these domains underlie the predictive relations between infant cognition and later language and IQ Colombo, 1993, Rose & Tamis-Lemonda, 1999. In addition, in view of the dynamic nature of early development, an effort was made to restrict the assessments of cognitive performance to a single age (7 months). In this way, we could avoid complicating any conclusions about the dimensionality of infant cognition with changes in cognitive structure that might occur over the first year of life. Multiple measures of each domain were obtained from a battery of tasks administered over two sessions.

The first domain, memory, is recognized as fundamental to learning and development. One type of memory, visual recognition, was one of the first aspects of infant cognition to show predictive validity for later cognitive ability Bornstein & Sigman, 1986, Fagan & Detterman, 1992, McCall & Carriger, 1993. We have included measures capturing additional aspects of memory in infancy, some of which were developed in the course of this project. One, a span task, assesses short-term memory capacity, an important component of working memory. Another, a task examining retention over successively longer delays, assesses more long-term memory.

Speed of processing is another aspect of infant cognition considered to have important implications for later intellectual functioning. The concept of processing speed has served as the cornerstone in many theoretical formulations about the nature of cognition Detterman, 1987, Jensen, 1992, Vernon, 1987 and has been found to be the central limiting factor in a wide range of diverse cognitive achievements in childhood and adolescence Hale, 1990, Kail, 1991. In the present study, reaction time (RT), one aspect of processing speed, was assessed in infancy with the Visual Expectation Task-VExP (Haith, Hazan, & Goodman, 1988). This task gauges ocular RT to a fast-paced sequence of computer-generated pictures that appear in a predictable (or unpredictable) spatiotemporal order. In addition to the VExP task, we included a continuous familiarization task that assesses a second aspect of processing speed—encoding time. In this task, infants are presented with a series of paired stimuli (in this case, pictures of faces), one of which remains the same from trial-to-trial, while the other changes Rose et al., 2002, Rose et al., 1999, Rose et al., 2002. Trials continue until the infant recognizes the unchanging picture, as gauged by the emergence of a consistent preference for the novel target.

Attention, a precondition for any cognitive activity, is frequently studied in infancy by examining look duration and shift rate. Growing evidence indicates that shorter looks and higher shift rates are indicative of superior attention. Look durations decrease and shift rates increase dramatically with age Colombo et al., 1987, Frick et al., 1999, Rose et al., 2001a, while, within an age, more mature patterns of attention (short looks and more shifts) are associated with better information processing and visual recognition memory Colombo, 1993, Colombo et al., 1991, Freeseman et al., 1993, Jankowski & Rose, 1997, Jankowski et al., 2001, Rose et al., 1999. Longer looks are characteristic of risk infants Carlson & Werkman, 1996, Jacobson et al., 1993, Reisbick et al., 1997, Rose et al., 2001a, Rose et al., 1988 and are associated with lower IQ Colombo et al., 2001, Rose et al., 1986, Sigman et al., 1991, Sigman et al., 1997.

Representational competence refers to the ability to extract, from concrete individual instances, information that is common to them and to represent this commonality in some abstract or symbolic form. The possibility that this type of mental representation could support some cognitive continuity was suggested by a study relating habituation in infancy to a latent dimension of representational competence in later childhood, comprised of language comprehension and play sophistication (Tamis-Lemonda & Bornstein, 1990). In the present study, tactual-visual cross-modal transfer and anticipations represented this domain. Cross-modal transfer involves visual recognition of a shape based on a representation formed from tactual experience. This infant ability has been found to relate to language and IQ in later childhood Rose & Feldman, 1995, Rose et al., 1988, to show continuity from infancy to 11 years (Rose, Feldman, Futterweit, & Jankowski, 1997) and to relate to later visuospatial skills (Rose & Feldman, 1995). Anticipations, or expectations about future events, require that the infant detect the underlying regularity in a sequence of events and extrapolate from past experience to the future. The measure of anticipations used here was taken from the VExP task. This measure also shows considerable stability during the latter half of the first year Canfield et al., 1997, Rose et al., 2002.

Section snippets

Participants

Participants were full- and preterm infants who were enrolled in a prospective, longitudinal study of cognitive development and were seen at several ages over the first 3 years of life (5, 7, 12, 24, and 36 months). The present report concerns the performance on tasks given at 7 months (the first age at which the complete battery was used).

Infants were recruited from births at two hospitals affiliated with the Albert Einstein College of Medicine. At 7 months, there were n=144 full terms and n

Preliminary considerations—creation of composite measures of attention

To have a reasonable ratio of participants to variables, it was desirable to reduce the number of variables. This could be most easily accomplished by combining the multiple measures of attention to create one composite for look duration and another for shift rate. Before creating such composites, we sought to determine whether the participant-to-variable ratio could be further enhanced by combining the data from the preterm and full-term samples.

Discussion

The major finding of this study is that infant cognition is better characterized as multifaceted, rather than as a single, omnibus ability. This conclusion is based on the results of a principal axis factor analysis, with varimax rotation performed on 11 measures of cognition obtained at 7 months. These measures were obtained from a variety of procedures, conducted in different settings, and even on different days. The analysis yielded three factors: one attention factor (look duration and

Acknowledgements

This research was supported in part by a Social and Behavioral Sciences Research Grant from the March of Dimes Birth Defects Foundation, by Grants HD 13810 and HD 01799 from the National Institutes of Health. We would like to thank Michelle Brown, Donna Marie Caro, Sharon Frisch, Melissa Goldberg, Iris Sher, and Tina Schmitt for their help in testing infants and scoring data. Correspondence concerning this article should be sent to Susan A. Rose, Departments of Pediatrics and Psychiatry,

References (68)

  • S.F Buck

    A method of estimation of missing values in multivariate data suitable for use with an electronic computer

    Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, B

    (1960)
  • R.L Canfield et al.

    Information processing through the first year of life

    Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development

    (1997)
  • S.E Carlson et al.

    A randomized trial of visual attention of preterm infants fed docosahexaenoic acid until two months

    Lipids

    (1996)
  • R.B Cattell

    Abilities: Their structure, growth, and action

    (1971)
  • J Colombo

    Infant cognition: Predicting later intellectual functioning

    (1993)
  • J Colombo et al.

    Individual differences in infant visual attention: Are short lookers faster processors or feature processors?

    Child Development

    (1991)
  • J Colombo et al.

    Infant visual attention in the paired-comparison paradigm: Test–retest and attention-performance relations

    Child Development

    (1988)
  • J Colombo et al.

    The stability of visual habituation during the first year of life

    Child Development

    (1987)
  • Colombo, J., Richman, W. A., Shaddy, D. J., & Maikranz, J. M. (2001). Varieties of attention in habituation. Paper...
  • N Cowan

    The development of memory in childhood

    (1997)
  • D.K Detterman

    Theoretical notions of intelligence and mental retardation

    American Journal of Mental Deficiency

    (1987)
  • A Diamond et al.

    Prefrontal cortex cognitive deficits in children treated early and continuously for PKU

    Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development

    (1997)
  • T.M Dougherty et al.

    Infant expectations and reaction time as predictors of childhood speed of processing and IQ

    Developmental Psychology

    (1997)
  • J.F Fagan et al.

    The Fagan test of infant intelligence

    (1989)
  • L.J Freeseman et al.

    Individual differences in infant visual attention: Four-month-olds' discrimination and generalization of global and local stimulus properties

    Child Development

    (1993)
  • J.E Frick et al.

    Individual and developmental differences in disengagement of fixation in early infancy

    Child Development

    (1999)
  • J.A Green

    Testing whether correlation matrices are different from each other

    Developmental Psychology

    (1992)
  • J.P Guilford

    The nature of human intelligence

    (1967)
  • M.M Haith et al.

    Expectation and anticipation of dynamic visual events by 3.5-month-old babies

    Child Development

    (1988)
  • M.M Haith et al.

    Stability of visual expectations at 3.0 months of age

    Developmental Psychology

    (1990)
  • S Hale

    A global developmental trend in cognitive processing speed

    Child Development

    (1990)
  • Hollingshead, A.B. (1975). The Four-Factor Index of Social Status. Unpublished manuscript, Yale University. (Available...
  • J.L Horn

    Organization of abilities and the development of intelligence

    Psychological Review

    (1968)
  • S.W Jacobson et al.

    Visual expectation and dimensions of infant information processing

    Child Development

    (1992)
  • Cited by (37)

    • Infant speech perception and cognitive skills as predictors of later vocabulary

      2021, Infant Behavior and Development
      Citation Excerpt :

      In the past few decades, a growing literature on infant cognition suggests that infant cognitive skills develop rapidly in the first year of life (Colombo, Shaddy, Richman, Maikranz, & Blaga, 2004; Fagan, 1984). This research also suggests that infant cognitive skills can be measured during infancy (Rose, 1989; Rose, Feldman, & Jankowski, 2004), and variability in performance on infant cognitive tasks has been linked to individual differences in general cognitive outcomes later in life (Fagan & McGrath, 1981; Fagan, 1982; Rose & Feldman, 1995). The domain-general view of child language development suggests that language learning also relies on a set of cognitive processes that may not be specific to language (Colombo et al., 2009; Fernald, Perfors, & Marchman, 2006; Rose et al., 2009).

    • What aspects of others' behaviors do infants attend to in live situations?

      2015, Infant Behavior and Development
      Citation Excerpt :

      Investigating what behaviors of others infants focus on, can give us information about when infants start to understand others’ actions (Kochukhova & Gredebäck, 2010; Woodward, 2005), and social relations (Augusti et al., 2010; Rakoczy, Warneken, & Tomasello, 2007; Stenberg & Hagekull, 2007; von Hofsten et al., 2009). To examine infant visual attention – preferential and selective looking – helps us gain insight into the development of infants’ perceptual and cognitive capabilities (Haith & Benson, 1998; Kellman & Banks, 1998) as well as infants’ developing ability to process information and social cues (e.g., Rose, Feldman, & Jankowski, 2004). Additionally, knowing what typically developing infants notice in social situations can be of great help to identify abnormalities in development.

    • Information processing from infancy to 11years: Continuities and prediction of IQ

      2012, Intelligence
      Citation Excerpt :

      Habituation was infant-controlled, with objects presented until the infant had a few looks away (ranging from three 3 s looks away at 7 months to two 1 s looks away at 36 months). Novelty scores were calculated for each problem and averaged over all 9 problems (Rose et al., 2004a). Measure: mean novelty score.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text