Early control of reaching: Effects of experience and body orientation

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2007.06.001Get rights and content

Abstract

Although research suggests that experience may be a better indicator of the acquisition of certain abilities by infants than age, little work addresses this issue in the development of reaching movements in particular. This longitudinal study fills this gap by verifying the effect of practice time on more- and less-skilled reachers of the same age group in different body orientations. Less- (n = 6) and more-skilled (n = 4) reachers were evaluated at the acquisition of reaching and again after 1 month of spontaneous practice. Kinematic analyses examined 3D wrist motion during reaching trials in supine (0°), reclined (45°) and seated (70°) orientations. The results indicated that skill level was a relevant factor in dealing with gravitational constraints imposed by different body orientations. Time of practice affected the way less- and more-skilled reachers explored and adapted the kinematic parameters of reaching to constraints imposed by body orientations. These findings suggest that not only age, but also experience and body orientations are important constraints that should be taken into account when examining infant reaching development.

Introduction

Infants acquire and develop important motor abilities during the first year of life. The emergence of reaching, in particular, has been studied extensively during the last thirty years (Konczak & Dichgans, 1997; Thelen, Corbetta, & Spencer, 1996; Von Hofsten, 1979, Von Hofsten, 1982, Von Hofsten, 1991). A reaching movement requires a certain level of interaction between infant and environment, and represents an improved perception of the world and recognition of some gamut of possible actions (Rosengren, Savelsbergh, & Van der Kamp, 2003; Savelsbergh & Van der Kamp, 1993; Von Hofsten, 1991). From a constraint-led perspective, the development of action is generally brought about by changes within and between the constraints imposed upon the organism–environment system (Newell, 1986). Particular constraints may act, at certain developmental time, as rate-limiting factors in the emergence and mastering of new motor actions. In other words, one can consider constraints as those factors that somehow set boundaries for the control and coordination of an action and changes therein. The interaction of these constraints does not prescribe the pattern or the performance of an action, but it guides its development by making the occurrence of certain patterns more or less probable. A change in the confluence of constraints, therefore, can eventuate in a change in action, with no predominant constraints. It is the interaction between intrinsic and extrinsic constraints that makes some but not other actions possible at a particular time and place (Rosengren et al., 2003). The present study examines how the interaction between intrinsic and extrinsic constraints affects the early coordination of reaching.

Newell's constraint-led perspective was supported by studies that attempted to understand the development of reaching by taking into account intrinsic constraints, like the level of postural control (Fallang, Saugstad, & Hadders-Algra, 2000; Rochat & Goubet, 1995), as well as extrinsic constraints, such as investigating the use of objects of different sizes (Van Hof, Van der Kamp, & Savelsbergh, 2006) and rigidity (Rocha, Silva, & Tudella, 2006), objects in motion (Van Hof, Van der Kamp, Caljouw, & Savelsbergh, 2005) and influence of gravity by imposing different body orientations (Rochat, 1992; Savelsbergh & Van der Kamp, 1994; Out, Van Soest, Savelsbergh, & Hopkins, 1998).

In the experiment conducted by Savelsbergh and Van der Kamp (1994), reaching movements were analyzed in 12–27-week-old infants positioned in an infant chair that could be adjusted to three body orientations: seated, reclined, and supine. Reaching and grasping were observed to be affected by age and body orientation. Specifically in the seated orientation, the 12–19-week-old infants showed patterns of reaching and grasping that were comparable to those of 20–27-week-old infants, regardless of body orientation. However, when the younger infants were in supine, they not only performed fewer reaching and grasping movements, but also seemed to have poorer control and coordination. The observation that only the young infants’ movements were affected by body orientation suggests that biomechanical constraints (e.g. muscle power in order to overcome the gravity force) may act as a rate-limiting factor during the development of reaching and grasping. Carvalho, Tudella and Savelsbergh (2007), when conducting a longitudinal study to verify the effect of seated and supine positions on spatio-temporal parameters of reaching at 4, 5 and 6 months, verified that frequency and straightness of reaching increased over age. Significant differences in reaching between the body orientations were observed at 4 months, that is, the frequency increased and the duration and deceleration time decreased in the seated position. There were no significant differences between the body orientations at 5 and 6 months. These findings suggest that young infants adapt their behavior to both intrinsic (i.e. age) and extrinsic (i.e. body orientation) constraints. In summary, this previous research linked adaptive response with infant's age, in which the latter is intercorrelated with developmental factors such as changes in body dimensions, duration of reaching experience, and reaching proficiency (Adolph, 1997). In this regard, it is important to note that Adolph's study (1997) on the locomotion over different slopes showed that the duration of crawling experience is a better predictor of adaptive response than age. Not only experience but also the infants’ level of skill may affect adaptiveness. Generally, more-skilled performers are better able to direct their attention to relevant properties, just like an expert rock climber may notice handholds and footholds that a novice would not (Gibson, 1969). Thus, infants’ reaching skill may also mediate adaptive responses. The literature provides scant data about effects of practice duration and skill on adaptive reaching. Previous work showed that older, probably more experienced and skilled reachers, behaved more adaptively than younger, probably less experienced and skilled reachers. The present study explores the possibility to investigate these factors separately in order to find whether practice time and infant's reaching skill are more important constraints than age.

There are many factors that can determine the level of reaching behavior in young infants, namely the number of opportunities to practice the movement, improvement in controlling arm movements, perception of the appropriate action for specific situations and inclination to reach (Lobo, Galloway, & Savelsbergh, 2004; Wimmers, Savelsbergh, Beek, & Hopkins, 1998; Van Hof et al., 2005). However, the question is: how do we determine that infants of the same age are more or less skilled in reaching? The works of Savelsbergh and Van der Kamp (1994) and Carvalho et al. (2007) provide us with a suggestion. Reaching movements were less controlled and coordinated in the infants that showed fewer reaches in the supine position. In contrast, better controlled reaching movements were observed in infants that showed more reaches in supine position. In other words, if an infant reaches more (i.e. a higher amount of reaches) in the supine body orientation, s/he is more-skilled in reaching. To classify infants as either less- or more-skilled reachers, we first identified the frequency of reaching in supine, reclined and seated orientations. Particularly the amount of reaching in supine was picked out as an indicator for the level of reaching skill.

The aim of the current exploratory study is to examine the effect of practice time on reaching movements for more- and less-skilled reachers in different body orientations. The experimental design adopted was longitudinal because it is well known that reaching does not emerge at the same age for every infant (Carvalho et al., 2007; Rocha et al., 2006; Thelen, Corbetta, Spencer, Schneider, & Zernicke (1993); Wimmers et al., 1998). The current experiment tracked infants’ reaching performance in different body orientations from their first reaching attempts to experienced reaching achieved several weeks later. Thus, the design controlled practice duration, but infants’ ages varied within test sessions. Anyway, all infants grew older across sessions. In order to determine the skill level at the acquisition of reaching, this behavior was evaluated not at a particular age, but at the ‘onset’ of reaching, i.e. at the first lab visit after the infants acquired the behavior.

Section snippets

Participants

Thirteen healthy full term infants (four boys and nine girls) participated in this study. The infants were evaluated longitudinally at the ages of 4 (M = 4 months 1 day; S.D. = 3 days), 5 (M = 5 months; S.D. = 2 days), and 6 months (M = 6 months; S.D. = 3 days). However, three infants were excluded from this study since they performed no reaches during two consecutive evaluations. Based on statistical analyses for frequency of reaches in supine, reclined and seated body orientations at the acquisition of

Results

Based on the frequency of reaches in supine body orientation at the acquisition of reaching (PT1), infants were classified into two categories: more- and less-skilled reachers (Table 1). The Chi-square test endorsed lower frequency of reaching in supine compared to other body orientations for the less-skilled group (X2(2) = 20.739; p < 0.01) and no difference in frequency of reaching among body orientations for more-skilled reachers (X2(2) = 1.28; p = 0.527) (see Fig. 1). In general, it was observed a

Discussion

The literature provides scant data about the effects of practice duration and skill level on adaptive reaching, frequently underlining that adaptive reaching is ascribed to infants’ age. Previous work showed that older, probably more experienced and skilled reachers, behaved more adaptively than younger, probably less experienced and skilled reachers. For example, significant differences in reaching behavior between seated and supine body orientations were observed at 4 months, and these

References (32)

  • P. Van Hof et al.

    The confluence of intrinsic and extrinsic constraints on 3- to 9-month-old infants’ catching behavior

    Infant Behavior & Development

    (2005)
  • P. Van Hof et al.

    Three to eight months infants cacthing under monocular and binocular vision

    Human Movement Science

    (2006)
  • Adolph, K. E. (1997). Learning in development of infant locomotion. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child...
  • R.M.L. Barros et al.

    Development and evaluation of a system for three-dimensional kinematic analysis of human movements. Desenvolvimento e avaliação de um sistema para análise cinemática tridimensional de movimentos humanos

    Revista Brasileira de Engenharia Biomédica

    (1999)
  • R.P. Carvalho et al.

    Utilization of the Dvideow system in kinematic analysis of infants’ reaching movements. Utilização do sistema Dvideow na análise cinemática do alcance manual de lactentes

    Brazilian Journal of Physical Therapy

    (2005)
  • D. Corbetta et al.

    The developmental origins of bimanual coordination: A dynamic perspective

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance

    (1996)
  • Cited by (56)

    • Does late preterm birth impact trunk control and early reaching behavior?

      2021, Infant Behavior and Development
      Citation Excerpt :

      The beginning of a reach was defined as the first frame when the infant’s arm began an uninterrupted movement toward the object. The end of a reach was defined as the first frame when the infant’s hand touched the object (Carvalho, Tudella, Caljouw, & Savelsbergh, 2008; Cunha et al., 2013; de A. Soares, van der Kamp, Savelsbergh, & Tudella, 2013; de Toledo et al., 2011). It was calculated as the number of reaches considered valid for 2 min in each sitting condition.

    • Brief reaching training with “sticky mittens” in preterm infants: Randomized controlled trial

      2019, Human Movement Science
      Citation Excerpt :

      Reaching behavior was observed frame by frame using images from the camera in a monitor. A valid reaching consisted of the movement of one or both hands toward the toy resulting in contact, regardless of grasping (Carvalho, Tudella, Caljouw, & Savelsbergh, 2008; Cunha et al., 2013; Cunha et al., 2013; Savelsbergh & Van Der Kamp, 1994; Soares et al., 2013; Toledo et al., 2011). The first frame when the infant started an uninterrupted movement of the hand toward the toy was defined as the beginning of a reach.

    • Physical Growth, Body Scale, and Perceptual-Motor Development

      2018, Advances in Child Development and Behavior
    • Object engagement and manipulation in extremely preterm and full term infants at 6 months of age

      2016, Research in Developmental Disabilities
      Citation Excerpt :

      Second, infants’ prolonged recovery in the NICU and after discharge (Als et al., 2004; Sansavini, Guarini & Caselli, 2011) may have negative effects on postural development (Van Haastert, de Vries, Helders, & Jongmans, 2006; Sansavini et al., 2014; Wijnroks & van Veldhoven, 2003), which over time may make exploration more difficult to execute. Postural control plays an important role in learning and goal oriented behaviour in infancy (Carvalho et al., 2008; Rochat & Bullinger, 1994). Infants who can sit alone have both hands free to move in the service of the object manipulation (e.g., Berthental & Von Hofsten, 1998; Soska & Adolph, 2014); and indeed, the quality of reaching movements improves when infants become proficient in sitting without support (Carvalho, Tudella & Savelsbergh, 2007; Rochat & Goubet, 1995).

    • Maturational and situational determinants of reaching at its onset

      2015, Infant Behavior and Development
      Citation Excerpt :

      Adjustment Index was calculated as the ratio between the time after peak velocity and the movement duration. This measures the percentage of time it takes to slow down the arm movement before touching the object (Carvalho et al., 2008). Straightness Index was defined as the ratio between the shortest distance between the beginning and the end of the reach and the actual distance traveled by the hand.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text