The validity of finger pulse line length for the detection of concealed information

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.07.015Get rights and content

Abstract

Building on the orienting theory of the Concealed Information Test, Elaad and Ben-Shakhar [Elaad, E., & Ben-Shakhar, G. (2006). Finger pulse waveform length in the detection of concealed information. Int. J. Psychophysiol., 61, 226–234.] proposed a new measure for the detection of concealed information: finger pulse line length (FPLL). This measure is recorded with a plethysmograph and combines pulse rate and pulse amplitude. Because the validity of finger pulse amplitude is still not established, one could question the benefits of FPLL to well validated measures such as heart rate. In the present study we compared the validity of FPLL, finger pulse rate and finger pulse amplitude in the detection of concealed information. Male community volunteers (n = 77) who had acquired crime knowledge through a mock crime procedure, were instructed to deny knowledge in a Concealed Information Test. Skin conductance, finger pulse line length, finger pulse rate and amplitude, heart rate and respiration were recorded. FPLL validity was significantly higher than that of finger pulse amplitude and heart rate. These data, together with its ease of measurement, support the use of FPLL.

Section snippets

Participants

Seventy-nine male community volunteers were recruited through an advertisement in local magazines. We only recruited male participants because in Belgium most crimes are committed by male offenders and most examinees of polygraph examinations are men. Volunteers who contacted us were allowed in the study if they were aged between 18 and 65 years old, and reported not taking medication for medical or psychiatric illnesses. In addition, we selected participants so that we obtained a sample that

Manipulation checks

Ninety-nine percent of the catch items was named correctly, indicating that the participants indeed paid attention to the pictures. Results from the memory check show that the crime pictures were correctly recognized and remembered. Ninety-eight percent of the crime pictures was correctly classified.

Video mock crime

We included mock crime as between subject factor in the initial analyses. No significant effect was found (F < 1.1). We therefore ignored this variable in further analyses.

Psychophysiological measures

Detection efficiency was

Discussion

The present study examined FPLL as an indicator of concealed information. With regard to our three research questions, our results can be summarized as follows.

First, ROC curve analyses showed that detection efficiency of FPA and PR was significantly above chance, but weak (ROCa = .60–.65). Despite their weak validity, combining them into FPLL lead to high detection accuracy (ROCa = .82). These results indicate that FPLL is a more sensitive measure than its components. We also examined the relative

References (47)

  • Ben-ShakharG.

    A critical review of the control question test

  • Ben-ShakharG. et al.

    Theories and Applications in the Detection of Deception: A Psychophysiological International Perspective

    (1990)
  • Ben-ShakharG. et al.

    Psychophysiological detection through the guilty knowledge technique: effects of mental countermeasures

    J. Appl. Psychol

    (1996)
  • Ben-ShakharG. et al.

    The validity of psychophysiological detection of information with the guilty knowledge test: a meta-analytic review

    J. Appl. Psychol.

    (2003)
  • Ben-ShakharG. et al.

    Leakage of relevant information to innocent examinees in the GKT: an attempt to reduce false-positive outcomes by introducing target stimuli

    J. Appl. Psychol.

    (1999)
  • Ben-ShakharG. et al.

    Orienting response reinstatement and dishabituation: effects of substituting, adding and deleting components of nonsignificant stimuli

    Psychophysiology

    (2000)
  • Ben-ShakharG. et al.

    Trial by polygraph: reconsidering the use of the guilty knowledge technique in court

    Law Hum. Behav.

    (2002)
  • BradleyM.T. et al.

    Alcohol and the psychophysiological detection of deception

    Psychophysiology

    (1984)
  • BradleyM.T. et al.

    Accuracy demonstrations, threat and the detection of deception: cardiovascular, electrodermal and papillary measures

    Psychophysiology

    (1981)
  • CarmelD. et al.

    Estimating the validity of the guilty knowledge test from simulated experiments: the external validity of mock crime studies

    J. Exp. Psychol. Appl.

    (2003)
  • CutrowR.J. et al.

    The objective use of multiple physiological indices in the detection of deception

    Psychophysiology

    (1972)
  • DawsonM.E. et al.

    The electrodermal system

  • De ClercqA. et al.

    Psychophysiological Analyses (PSPHA): a modular script-based program for analyzing psychophysiological data

    Behav. Res. Meth.

    (2006)
  • Cited by (27)

    • Physiological Measures in the Detection of Deception and Concealed Information

      2018, Detecting Concealed Information and Deception: Recent Developments
    • Effects of goal- and task-oriented motivation in the guilty action test

      2013, International Journal of Psychophysiology
      Citation Excerpt :

      From here on, the term RLL refers to covert respiration back recordings. Finger pulse waveform length (FPWL) responses were suggested as a useful addition to the measures currently employed in the detection of concealed information (Ambach et al., 2012; Elaad and Ben-Shakhar, 2006; Vandenbosch et al., 2009). Finger pulse data were recorded using an Atlas Researches (Israel) piezoelectric plethysmograph positioned around the right thumb.

    • Improved method for calculating the respiratory line length in the Concealed Information Test

      2011, International Journal of Psychophysiology
      Citation Excerpt :

      Although these two methods analyze only the respiration curve after the item onset, the part of the respiratory cycle before the item onset would affect the remaining part of the cycle, which is included for calculating the respiration line length. However, the CIT often uses a rather long inter-stimulus interval (i.e., more than 20 s; for example see Gamer et al., 2008; Vandenbosch et al., 2009; Verschuere et al., 2007). This time is set so that a respiratory (and heart rate) response to a question item recovers to the base level (for the time course of the respiratory response after a question item onset, see Fig. 2 of Matsuda et al., 2009a and Fig. 1 of Matsuda et al., 2009b).

    • An interfering n-back task facilitates the detection of concealed information with EDA but impedes it with cardiopulmonary physiology

      2011, International Journal of Psychophysiology
      Citation Excerpt :

      The Concealed Information Test (CIT) originally called Guilty Knowledge Test by Lykken (1959) is a systematic and standardized test procedure employed to detect hidden knowledge by means of physiological recordings. The relevance and validity of the different physiological measures for the detection of concealed knowledge are still a matter of ongoing research (see Elaad et al., 1992; Elaad and Ben-Shakhar, 2006; Hirota et al., 2003, Vandenbosch et al., 2009). A typical investigation procedure combines a standardized interrogation with the continuous collection of electrodermal activity (EDA), electrocardiogram (ECG), breathing activity, and finger plethysmogram.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    1

    The first two authors contributed equally to the manuscript.

    2

    Bruno Verschuere is a postdoctoral fellow of the Research Foundation – Flanders (FWO).

    View full text