Elsevier

Gait & Posture

Volume 26, Issue 2, July 2007, Pages 231-237
Gait & Posture

The effect of a concurrent task on the walking performance of preschool children

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gaitpost.2006.09.004Get rights and content

Abstract

Forty-eight children, aged 4–6 years, walked while performing or not performing an easy or difficult concurrent motor task (carrying a tray with or without marbles on it) or cognitive task (repeating a series of digits forwards or backwards). The temporal-distance gait parameters were measured using a GAITRite electronic walkway system. Dual-task costs were calculated as the difference between the children's walking performances with and without the concurrent task. Overall, walking alone was easier than walking while performing a concurrent task. A greater dual-task cost was observed when the concurrent task was difficult compared to when it was easy. However, this effect of difficulty was found for the motor task only. The results suggest that walking demands both the central processing capacity and the domain- and modality-specific processing resources in normal 4–6-year-old children.

Introduction

Walking has traditionally been considered to be an automatic activity without the involvement of cognitive processing. However, this concept has been challenged by the results of recent studies. Through a dual-task paradigm in which participants were asked to perform two tasks at the same time, these studies showed that walking was affected by a concurrent task, indicating that walking, in fact, required attention [1], [2], [3], [4], [5], [6]. These studies also suggested that the effect of a concurrent task on walking depended on the type of task and its level of difficulty [1], [7], [8]. The effect of a concurrent task on walking was also found to be greater in elderly participants or in participants with postural control problems [8], [9]. As yet, little is known about the dual-task effect in children.

Whitall [10] examined the effects of concurrent cognitive tasks on running and galloping in female children ranging in age from 2.5 to 10 years, as well as in young female adults. The cognitive tasks were vocal singing and non-vocal memorization. The researcher found that both tasks interfered with gait speed in all age groups. For the 4 and the 6 years old, decrease in gait speed was more pronounced when the concurrent task was non-vocal memorization. The decrease in running gait speed caused by non-vocal memorization manifested itself in step length, while that caused by vocal singing manifested itself in both step time and step length.

Huang et al. [11] found that the effect of a concurrent task on gait was task-related. Comparing the effects of different cognitive tasks (visual identification, auditory identification and memorization) on gait variables in typically developing 5–7-year-old children, the researchers found that the auditory identification task caused the greatest interference in gait speed, cadence and step length, while memorization caused the smallest interference and only in speed and cadence. Huang and her colleagues also acknowledged that these differential effects could be due to the different difficulty levels of the tasks, which they failed to control.

The locomotion tasks studied by Whitall [10] and Huang et al. [11] were running, galloping and fast walking. These are more advanced, more balance-challenging and less automatic locomotor tasks than free walking, the latter being considered by intuition as easy and automatic. We were interested in knowing whether a concurrent task might also affect free walking in children.

Most children, by 3.5 years of age, have achieved an adult-like walking pattern (e.g. reciprocal arm swing) [12]. However, gait and balance are not fully developed until 7 years of age or even older [12], [13]. It is also reported that 7-year-old children still demonstrate high stride-to-stride variability [14]. Thus, because walking is still developing in children under 7 years, it is conceivable that walking may be vulnerable to interference from a concurrently performed task. Here, we report a study that examined the effect of a concurrent task on walking in typically developing 4–6-year-old preschool children. Two types of concurrent tasks were employed, one motor and the other cognitive. Two levels of difficulty were defined within each type of task (easy and difficult). We hypothesized that preschool children's walking would be affected by the concurrent tasks and that the effect would depend on the type as well as the difficulty level of the task.

Section snippets

Participants

Forty-eight typically developing preschool children (26 girls and 22 boys, mean age = 60.1 months, standard deviation (S.D.) = 6.9) participated in the study. They were recruited from a local preschool. The children were free of any developmental delay, and any neurological, orthopedic or cardiovascular condition. They were also mentally equivalent with peers, according to their teachers’ reports and were without motor coordination deficits, as assessed by the Movement Assessment Battery for

Results

Table 1 presents the means and S.D.s for each gait parameter when the participants walked with no concurrent task (free walking), with a concurrent easy or difficult motor task, and with a concurrent easy or difficult cognitive task. One-way repeated-measure ANOVA revealed a significant effect on the walking condition for all gait parameters (speed: F4,188 = 102.11, p < 0.0001; stride length: F4,188 = 136.09, p < 0.0001; cadence: F4,188 = 42.87, p < 0.0001; double limb support: F4,188 = 83.06, p < 0.0001; base

Discussion

The present study examined the effects of concurrent motor and cognitive tasks on the usual walking performance of typically developing preschool children. In general, the results showed that normal free walking, a very basic and highly practiced task, was affected when the children had to carry out a concurrent task. The concurrent task decreased speed, cadence and stride length, and increased double limb support percentage and base of support. These results were consistent with those of Huang

Acknowledgements

The authors wish to express their sincere gratitude to the participating children, their families and teachers. The study was supported by grants from the National Science Council, Taiwan, R.O.C. under grant no. NSC 94-2614-E-006-077 and NSC 95-2221-E-006-015-MY2.

References (26)

  • G. Ebersbach et al.

    Influence of concurrent tasks on gait: a dual-task approach

    Percept Mot Skills

    (1995)
  • Y. Lajoie et al.

    Attentional demands for static and dynamic equilibrium

    Exp Brain Res

    (1993)
  • Y. Lajoie et al.

    Upright standing and gait: are there changes in attentional requirements related to normal aging?

    Exp Aging Res

    (1996)
  • A. Shumway-Cook et al.

    The effects of two types of cognitive tasks on postural stability in older adults with and without a history of falls

    J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci

    (1997)
  • N. Teasdale et al.

    On the cognitive penetrability of postural control

    Exp Aging Res

    (1993)
  • M. Woollacott et al.

    Attention and the control of posture and gait: a review of an emerging area of research

    Gait Posture

    (2002)
  • O. Beauchet et al.

    Dual-task-related gait changes in the elderly: does the type of cognitive task matter?

    J Mot Behav

    (2005)
  • J.M. Bond et al.

    Goal-directed secondary motor tasks: their effects on gait in subjects with Parkinson disease

    Arch Phys Med Rehabil

    (2000)
  • T.M. Parker et al.

    The effect of divided attention on gait stability following concussion

    Clin Biomech (Bristol, Avon)

    (2005)
  • J. Whitall

    The developmental effect of concurrent cognitive and locomotor skills: time-sharing from a dynamic perspective

    J Exp Child Psychol

    (1991)
  • H.J. Huang et al.

    Effects of different concurrent cognitive tasks on temporal-distance gait variables in children

    Pediatr Phys Ther

    (2003)
  • D.H. Sutherland et al.

    The development of mature gait

    J Bone Joint Surg Am

    (1980)
  • R. Norlin et al.

    Development of gait in the normal child

    J Pediatr Orthop

    (1981)
  • Cited by (48)

    • Attentional load of walking in children aged 7–12 and in adults

      2017, Gait and Posture
      Citation Excerpt :

      In a recent study, Gazzellini et al. [28] showed that visual processes in children are used as anticipatory control mechanisms of gait, which may suggest that children rely more than adults on these anticipatory control mechanisms because gait is not fully mature at 12 years of age. These findings are also consistent with the hypothesis that different dual-tasks induce a different effect of interference [5,15] and that this interference decreases from 4 to 16 years of age, for a review, see Ref. [13]. To summarize, the main issue addressed in this experiment was to further identify the attentional demand of walking during ontogenesis.

    • The developmental dynamics of gait maturation with a focus on spatiotemporal measures

      2017, Gait and Posture
      Citation Excerpt :

      For example, Huang et al. [115] found that in 5–7 year old children, visual identification and sound identification dual-tasks were associated with decreased cadence and step length, while a memory task affected just cadence. Moreover, Cherng et al. [108] found that in 4–6 year olds both motor and cognitive dual-tasks affected velocity, cadence and base of support, while only the motor tasks were associated with a reduction in stride length and increased percentage of double support time. Importantly, while changes in attentional capacity and executive functions are likely to play a role here [116], this has not been specifically investigated with respect to the different components of executive function (Huizinga et al., 2006).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text