The development of maternal touch across the first year of life

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2007.09.019Get rights and content

Abstract

The developmental trajectories of specific forms of maternal touch during natural caregiving were examined across the first year in relation to the development of mother–infant reciprocal communication. One hundred and thirty-one mothers and infants in four groups aged 3, 6, 9, and 12 months were observed in a cross-sectional design at home during natural caregiving and mother–child play sessions. Microanalytic coding of the caregiving sessions considered nine forms of maternal touch, which were aggregated into three global touch categories: affectionate, stimulating, and instrumental. Play sessions were coded for maternal sensitivity and dyadic reciprocity. Maternal affectionate and stimulating touch decreased significantly during the second 6 months of life. In parallel, dyadic reciprocity increased in the second half year. Dyadic reciprocity was predicted by the frequency of affectionate touch but not by any other form of touch. Results contribute to specifying the role of touch as it evolves across the first year of life within the global mother–infant communication system.

Introduction

Touch is a multi-dimensional phenomenon that includes various sub-types, each of which may affect infant development in a unique way, especially during the first months of life. For instance, higher levels of maternal affectionate touch in early infancy were found to predict cognitive and neurobehavioral development [1], [2], better maternal adaptation to the mothering role [3], and an increase in infant smiles and vocalizations [4]. On the other hand, higher levels of stimulating touch elicited more negative facial expressions compared with stroking and holding [5], [6]. Mothers tend to use different forms of touch as a result of infant cues and temperamental dispositions [7], cultural background [8], or in situations where the typical pattern of mother–infant relatedness is disrupted, such as during the still-face paradigm.

Individual differences in maternal well-being are also expressed through the frequency and type of maternal touch. For example, depressed mothers tend to use less affectionate touch and more stimulating touch [1], [9], [10], [11], [12]. Due to the fact that touch is sensitive to both maternal and infant contributions, it is possible that different forms of maternal touch may have unique effects at different stages of development and thus, the development of specific forms of maternal touch and their role within the mother–infant normal relational system should be investigated across infancy [13].

Previous studies of maternal touch typically differentiated various forms of touch using global constructs, such as harsh or negative touch versus affectionate touch (i.e., caressing, holding, and light stroking versus harsh, awkward and forcing; [14]). More detailed inventories of touch subcategories include proprioceptive touch, vestibular touch, passive touch, instrumental touch, and firm touch [15], [16], [9], [11], [17], [18]. In combination, the various forms of positive maternal touch types may be grouped into three central subcategories: affectionate touch, stimulating touch, and instrumental touch. Each of these sub-types has been used in at least one of the aforementioned studies to differentiate healthy from at-risk dyads.

Studies of maternal touch in typically-developing infants have mainly focused on the first months of life (e.g. [19]). In contrast, studies of touch at older ages concentrated on pathological populations, such as children with feeding disorders or failure to thrive [16], [29], [15]. As both the frequency and quality of maternal touch in primates show substantial variations with development [20], it is important to examine the development of maternal spontaneous touch at different nodes across infancy in the normal human mother.

Maternal touch is part of the global mother–infant communicative system and is related to the level of maternal sensitivity and the degree of reciprocity and synchrony between mother and child. Reciprocity considers the degree of give and take between the mother and the child during the interaction and synchrony describes the temporal coordination between the behaviors of the mother and the child [21], [22], [23]. Reciprocity and synchrony develop across the first year of life [24], [25], [26], [27] and reflect the degree of familiarity mother and child have of each other's patterns and rhythms [29]. Reciprocity tends to develop across the first year of life whereas the degree of maternal sensitivity often remains constant, as sensitivity implies the mother's careful adaptation to the child's changing, age-appropriate signals and emerging capacities [29], [30], [31], [32]. However, the associations between various forms of maternal spontaneous touch during basic caregiving and the level of dyadic reciprocity at play across the first year of life have not yet been addressed. Because the mother's touch may evolve with time and its interactive meaning may change with development, it is important to chart how the three forms of maternal touch – affectionate, instrumental, and stimulating – interact with the give-and-take reciprocity between mother and child at different stages across the first year. Furthermore, charting the development of maternal touch across the first year in a normative sample may serve as a basis for understanding high-risk development. As such, the goal of this study was to provide normative data on the development of specific forms of maternal touch, focusing on concrete micro-level touch patterns during mother–infant interactions rather than on ecological factors such as maternal breastfeeding or infant temperament. Reciprocity and synchrony are among the most important components of the early caregiving environment and carry long-term effects on infant growth [25], and were thus chosen as the outcome variables.

In light of the above, the goals of the present study were twofold: (1) to examine developmental changes in maternal touch across the first year and (2) to address the role of touch categories in shaping the reciprocity between mother and infant, controlling for demographic maternal and infant variables that are known to affect the level of interactive synchrony. On the basis of previous research, we hypothesized that affectionate touch would be the most growth-promoting form of touch and would have the closest relations to mother–infant reciprocity as compared to all other forms of touch. Reciprocity was selected as an outcome variable due to the reported links between mother–infant reciprocity with the child's cognitive, linguistic and social–emotional outcomes [33], [34].

Section snippets

Participants

Participants in this cross-sectional study included 131 Israeli infants and their mothers in four groups; aged 3 (n = 43), 6 (n = 38), 9 (n = 28) and 12 months (n = 22). Infants at each age were recruited through Well-Baby clinics, a nation-wide childcare system that provides preventive medicine and developmental follow-up to nearly all Israeli infants. All infants received their developmental follow-up at the Israeli nation-wide Well-Baby clinics and were screened for developmental delays by nurses

Results

All variables were checked for normalcy conditions and all variables were normally distributed. The developmental trajectory of each form of touch – affectionate, stimulating, and instrumental – was examined using ANOVA. Following mean-level differences between two consecutive ages were examined with post-hoc comparisons with Duncan's tests.

Means, SDs, F values, and Effect Sizes (ES) for the three forms of touch across the different ages appear in Table 2.

As seen in Table 2, all forms of touch

Discussion

Results of this study indicated that the amount of maternal touch of all forms, including stimulating, affectionate, and instrumental touch, decrease during the first year of life. The data also suggest that mothers who tend to touch more are likely to use all forms of touch more frequently than those who provide less touch, findings that underscore the multi-dimensionality of touch. While we found small, but significant differences between age groups in the decrease of various forms of touch,

References (48)

  • M. Keren et al.

    Clinical interview for high-risk parents of premature infants (CLIP) as a predictor of early disruptions in the mother–infant relationship at the nursery

    Infant Ment Health J

    (2003)
  • D.M. Stack et al.

    Adult tactile stimulation during face-to-face interactions modulates five-month-olds' affect and attention

    Child Dev

    (1992)
  • E.Z. Tronick

    Touch in mother–infant interaction

  • L.M. Richter et al.

    Enrollment into birth to ten population and sample characteristics

    Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol

    (1995)
  • S.G. Ferber

    Some developmental facets of silence: a case study of a struggle to have a proximity figure

    Br J Psychother

    (2004)
  • S. Stepakoff et al.

    Mother–infant tactile communication at four month: infant gender, maternal ethnicity, and maternal depression

  • M.K. Weinberg et al.

    The impact of maternal psychiatric illness on infant development

    J Clin Psychiatry

    (1998)
  • D.W. Muir

    Adult communications with infants through touch: the forgotten sense

    Hum Dev

    (2002)
  • M.J. Hertenstein

    Touch: its communicative functions in infancy

    Hum Dev

    (2002)
  • S.J. Weiss

    Psychophysiologic and behavioral effects of tactile stimulation on infants with congenital heart disease

    Res Nurs Health

    (1992)
  • T. Field

    Early interventions for infants of depressed mothers

    Pediatrics

    (1998)
  • G. Schino et al.

    Infant handling and maternal response in Japanese macaques

    Int J Primatol

    (2003)
  • R. Feldman

    Infant–mother and infant–father synchrony: the coregulation of positive arousal

    Infant Ment Health J

    (2003)
  • R. Feldman et al.

    Testing a family intervention hypothesis: the contribution of mother–infant skin-to-skin contact (kangaroo care) to family interaction, proximity, and touch

    J Fam Psychol

    (2003)
  • Cited by (120)

    • Prenatal and postnatal intimate partner violence, depression, and infant-mother touch

      2022, Infant Behavior and Development
      Citation Excerpt :

      Mothers use touch to respond to infant needs, communicate safety, and teach infants how to regulate and care for themselves (e.g., Ciaunica & Crucianelli, 2019). Though infants touch their mothers less than vice versa, infants increasingly initiate touch interactions by one year of age to communicate their needs and internal states to their mothers (Hertenstein, 2002; Ferber, Feldman, & Makhoul, 2008; Kaye & Fogel, 1980). However, infant-mother interactive touch behavior may be disrupted by risk factors, such as maternal depression or intimate partner violence (IPV) that interfere with mother-infant contact, self-regulatory behavior, and the psychological or physiological perception of touch (e.g., D'Andrea, Pole, DePierro, Freed & Wallace, 2013; Feldman, Keren, Gross-Rozval, & Tyano, 2004; Herrera, Reissland, & Shepherd, 2004).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text