Elsevier

Clinical Psychology Review

Volume 49, November 2016, Pages 28-40
Clinical Psychology Review

Review
Inhibitory learning approaches to exposure therapy: A critical review and translation to obsessive-compulsive disorder

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2016.07.001Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Habituation as an indicator of improvement could have unintended consequences.

  • Inhibitory learning theory can be applied to treating OCD and its heterogeneity.

  • Introducing “desirable difficulties” into exposure may maximize long-term outcome.

  • Research translating inhibitory learning theory to exposure is in its nascent stage.

Abstract

The majority of treatment research on OCD has focused on pre/post treatment efficacy of exposure-based interventions, with less attention directed towards (a) understanding mechanisms of change, and (b) maximizing long-term effectiveness. Inhibitory learning theory (ILT) provides a novel foundation for understanding how exposure therapy reduces fear. Moreover, ILT is consistent with empirical evidence that raises questions about the more traditional (i.e., habituation) explanation for exposure therapy's efficacy. Yet ILT has yet to be applied to understanding the treatment of OCD and its heterogeneity. The current review is an examination of human experimental research on ILT that seeks to translate laboratory findings on fear extinction to exposure therapy across empirically established OCD symptom dimensions. We provide an up-to-date critical review of the existing evidence for a series of strategies derived from ILT that have been proposed for the treatment of fear, discuss the limitations of existing studies, and provide suggestions for future research within this rapidly accelerating area of study. We also offer conceptual considerations for applying these principles to the treatment of OCD symptom dimensions. A common theme is the idea of introducing “desirable difficulties” into the implementation of exposure in order to foster more durable long-term learning.

Introduction

Exposure and response prevention (ERP) is the most efficacious psychological treatment for obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD; e.g., Olatunji, Davis, Powers, & Smits, 2012). This intervention involves helping patients confront stimuli that provoke obsessional fear, but that objectively pose a low risk of harm. Exposure can occur in the form of actual encounters with feared situations or stimuli (situational or in vivo exposure), or in the form of imagined confrontation with the feared consequences of engaging with these stimuli (imaginal exposure). For example, an individual with obsessional fears that she unknowingly hit a pedestrian while driving her car would practice driving through crowded streets for situational exposure, and then confront doubts that she hit someone and could be held responsible. The response prevention component of ERP entails refraining from compulsive rituals and other behaviors that serve as an escape from obsessional fear. In the previous example, the patient would resist checking the roadside or the local news for reassurance that no such accidents have occurred.

Randomized controlled studies from around the world (e.g., Foa et al., 2005) indicate that ERP is more effective than credible control interventions (e.g., relaxation, anxiety management, pill placebo), with large pre-post effect sizes (e.g., Hedges's g = 1.39 in Olatunji et al., 2012), and with a substantial percentage of patients attaining clinically-significant improvement (e.g. 69% in Eddy, Dutra, Bradley, & Westen, 2004). Despite these outcomes, a sizeable percentage of patients (14–31%) are classified as non-responders (Foa et al., 2005, Norberg et al., 2008), and of those who respond, up to 50–60% experience at least partial relapse at later follow-up assessments (Eisen et al., 2013, Simpson et al., 2005). While some authors (e.g., Whittal, Thordarson, & McLean, 2005) suggest abandoning ERP altogether on the basis of these statistics (and the perceived “intolerability” of exposure exercises as described in Meyer, Farrell, Kemp, Blakey, & Deacon, 2014), an alternative approach is to focus efforts on improving the short- and long-term outcomes of this otherwise effective intervention.

In line with this latter option, Craske et al. (2008) have highlighted limitations of the long-standing and widely accepted idea that exposure therapy works by breaking conditioned fear responses via habituation (i.e., Emotional Processing Theory, EPT; e.g., Kozak & Foa, 1997). In contrast, laboratory research on fear extinction indicates that these associations do not break or disappear; rather exposure therapy leads to the learning of new non-threat (i.e., inhibitory) associations that compete with (rather than “break”) older threat associations. An important aim of exposure therapy, therefore, is to promote the encoding and long-term recall of the newly learned non-threat connections so that they will inhibit fear-based learning; a process termed “inhibitory learning”.

Research examining various facets and applications of inhibitory learning has accumulated in the last few decades, with some inconsistencies in findings across studies. Since the introduction of this area in 2008, however, there have been no comprehensive reviews critiquing this body of empirical work. Moreover, the recent experimental and clinical literature testing a priori hypotheses related to the inhibitory learning approach has focused almost exclusively on specific fears (e.g., spiders, heights, public speaking). Yet when the aforementioned room for improvement in ERP outcomes is considered along with the potential of the inhibitory learning approach, one recognizes the promise of translating this approach to the treatment of OCD. The aim of the current paper, therefore, is threefold. First, we highlight the major tenants, advantages, and confines of both the emotional processing and inhibitory learning accounts for how ERP is applied in the treatment of OCD. Second, we present a critical review of the existing human literature addressing implications of inhibitory learning for exposure therapy. While there exists a comprehensive animal literature on extinction learning and return of fear that parallels basic human laboratory research (Milad and Quirk, 2012, Milad et al., 2006, Phelps et al., 2004, Quirk and Mueller, 2008), there have frequently been contradictory findings when comparing animal and human studies (e.g., Thomas and Ayres, 2004, Vervliet et al., 2007). Accordingly, this review will reference important research using animal populations, but focus primarily on translational research using human participants. Third, we translate the inhibitory learning literature to the use of ERP for OCD. OCD is a heterogeneous condition requiring adaptation of ERP in various ways. Specifically, research has identified four theme-based symptom dimensions (Abramowitz et al., 2010, McKay et al., 2004), including: (a) contamination obsessions and decontamination rituals, (b) obsessions about being responsible for harm and checking rituals, (c) obsessions and rituals related to symmetry, completeness, and the need for things to seem “just right”, and (d) unacceptable obsessional thoughts (e.g., concerning violence, sex, or religion) and mental neutralizing rituals. Thus, we address the translation of research on inhibitory learning to the application of ERP for these various OCD presentations.

Section snippets

Emotional processing theory

Emotional processing theory (EPT; Foa et al., 2006, Foa and Kozak, 1986, Foa and McNally, 1996) has traditionally been the dominant model for explaining improvement during ERP (Abramowitz, Deacon, & Whiteside, 2011); although see Hyman and Pedrick (2010) for an exception. This theory proposes that therapeutic exposure must activate a “fear structure” that is contained in memory, and then provide information that is incompatible with the fear structure. This incompatible information is thought

The new theory of disuse: fear expression vs. fear learning

Bjork and Bjork (1992)’s New Theory of Disuse proposes that learned associations remain in memory even with “disuse.” In other words, once they are learned, such associations don't fade over time; rather access to them does. While generally adaptive, this process can be problematic for those with fear-based problems such as OCD, as the persistence of fear-based associations (even with disuse) leaves them vulnerable to relapse. In applying this theory to exposure therapy, Bjork and Bjork (2006)

Overview of the literature review

We next turn to a critical review of the empirical evidence regarding procedures that have been proposed based on ILT for the treatment of fear more generally (Craske et al., 2008). These procedures have in common the aim to optimize inhibitory learning in order to maximize long-term outcomes. Interest in this area is growing at a rapid pace and numerous translational studies have been conducted in the last few decades. For this review, we have a specific focus on how extinction of fear in

Definition and theoretical rationale

Rescorla and Wagner (Rescorla and Wagner, 1972, Wagner and Rescorla, 1972) proposed that new information is learned when there is a discrepancy between what is predicted and what actually occurs; and that the element of surprise is critical to the learning process (Rescorla, 1988). Thus, when an individual expects a negative outcome in response to a fear trigger, and these expectancies are violated during exposure (i.e., feared consequences either do not occur or are more manageable than

Definition and theoretical rationale

The Rescorla-Wagner model (1972) predicts that another method for introducing desirable difficulties (and thus promoting inhibitory learning) into exposure therapy is to combine multiple fear cues during exposure trials, as opposed to only presenting one fear cue at a time. Combining fear cues is thought to increase the discrepancy between fear-based expectations and actual outcomes. In other words, when an expected feared outcome does not occur despite the simultaneous presence of multiple

Definition and theoretical rationale

As previously mentioned, context renewal refers to a return of the conditioned fear response as a result of a change in context after extinction. For example, consider a patient whose fear of knives appears to undergo extinction after multiple in-session exposures; yet when she handles a knife at home in the presence of her newborn infant, her fear of knives is rekindled. Numerous studies show that, indeed (perhaps for evolutionary reasons), fear learning easily generalizes across contexts, yet

Definition and theoretical rationale

There are theoretical reasons to support the hypothesis that expanding the time interval between exposure sessions (Bjork & Bjork, 2006) is beneficial for long-term outcome. Massed practice (i.e., closely spaced learning episodes), on the one hand, may produce higher initial levels of retrieval strength and thus better short-term performance. For example, cramming all day may be useful if a student has an exam the next day. However, research suggests that information learned during massed

Conclusions, limitations, and future directions

Exposure-based therapy is the most empirically supported intervention for problems in which pathological fear is maintained by exaggerated beliefs about danger that lead to maladaptive escape/avoidance behavior. Craske et al. (2008) have pointed to limitations of the traditional EPT framework for understanding how exposure therapy works and how it can be implemented. On the basis of basic research on fear extinction, they propose a framework for exposure in which inhibitory learning is

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to thank Dr. Donald Baucom, Dr. David Penn, and Benjamin Buck who provided comments and suggestions on an early draft of this manuscript.

References (103)

  • CraskeM.G. et al.

    Maximizing exposure therapy: An inhibitory learning approach

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (2014)
  • CulverN.C. et al.

    Emotional variability and sustained arousal during exposure

    Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry

    (2012)
  • DeaconB. et al.

    Maximizing the efficacy of interoceptive exposure by optimizing inhibitory learning: A randomized controlled trial

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (2013)
  • EddyK.T. et al.

    A multidimensional meta-analysis of psychotherapy and pharmacotherapy for obsessive-compulsive disorder

    Clinical Psychology Review

    (2004)
  • GraysonJ.B. et al.

    Habituation during exposure treatment: Distraction vs. attention-focusing

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (1982)
  • KircanskiK. et al.

    Challenges to the traditional exposure paradigm: Variability in exposure therapy for contamination fears

    Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry

    (2012)
  • KozakM.J. et al.

    Process and outcome of exposure treatment with obsessive-compulsives: Psychophysiological indicators of emotional processing

    Behavior Therapy

    (1988)
  • LaderM.H. et al.

    A physiological model of phobic anxiety and desensitization

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (1968)
  • LangA.J. et al.

    Manipulations of exposure-based therapy to reduce return of fear: A replication

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (2000)
  • LovibondP.F. et al.

    Protection from extinction in human fear conditioning

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (2000)
  • MathewsA. et al.

    Emotional arousal and persuasion effects in flooding

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (1973)
  • McKayD. et al.

    A critical evaluation of obsessive-compulsive disorder subtypes: Symptoms versus mechanisms

    Clinical Psychology Review

    (2004)
  • MeyerJ.M. et al.

    Why do clinicians exclude anxious clients from exposure therapy?

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (2014)
  • MiladM.R. et al.

    Fear extinction in rats: Implications for human brain imaging and anxiety disorders

    Biological Psychology

    (2006)
  • MystkowskiJ.L. et al.

    Treatment context and return of fear in spider phobia

    Behavior Therapy

    (2002)
  • OlatunjiB.O. et al.

    Differential habituation of fear and disgust during repeated exposure to threat-relevant stimuli in contamination-based OCD: An analogue study

    Journal of Anxiety Disorders

    (2009)
  • ParkinsonL. et al.

    Are intrusive thoughts subject to habituation?

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (1980)
  • PhelpsE.A. et al.

    Extinction learning in humans: Role of the amygdala and vmPFC

    Neuron

    (2004)
  • RabavilasA.D. et al.

    Duration of flooding sessions in the treatment of obsessive-compulsive patients

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (1976)
  • RachmanS.J.

    The return of fear

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (1979)
  • RachmanS.J.

    The return of fear: Review and prospect

    Clinical Psychology Review

    (1989)
  • RachmanS.J. et al.

    Does escape behavior strengthen agoraphobic avoidance? A replication

    Behavior Therapy

    (1986)
  • RoweM.K. et al.

    Effects of an expanding-spaced vs. massed exposure schedule on fear reduction and return of fear

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (1998)
  • RoweM.K. et al.

    Effects of varied-stimulus exposure training on fear reduction and return of fear

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (1998)
  • ThomasB.L. et al.

    Use of the ABA fear renewal paradigm to assess the effects of extinction with co-present fear inhibitors or excitors: Implications for theories of extinction and for treating human fears and phobias

    Learning and Motivation

    (2004)
  • TsaoJ.C.I. et al.

    Timing of treatment and return of fear: Effects of massed, uniform-, and expanding-spaced exposure schedules

    Behavior Therapy

    (2000)
  • TwohigM.P. et al.

    Exposure therapy for OCD from an acceptance and commitment therapy (ACT) framework

    Journal of Obsessive-Compulsive and Related Disorders

    (2015)
  • VansteenwegenD. et al.

    Return of fear in a human differential conditioning paradigm caused by a return to the original acquistion context

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (2005)
  • VansteenwegenD. et al.

    The repeated confrontation with videotapes of spiders in multiple contexts attenuates renewal of fear in spider-anxious students

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (2007)
  • VervlietB. et al.

    Return of fear in a human differential conditioning paradigm caused by a stimulus change after extinction

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (2005)
  • VervlietB. et al.

    Concurrent excitors limit the extinction of conditioned fear in humans

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (2007)
  • AbramowitzJ.S. et al.

    Assessment of obsessive-compulsive symptom dimensions: Development and evaluation of the dimensional obsessive-compulsive scale

    Psychological Assessment

    (2010)
  • AbramowitzJ.S. et al.

    Exposure therapy for anxiety: Principles and practice

    (2011)
  • AbramowitzJ.S. et al.

    Exposure and ritual prevention for obsessive-compulsive disorder: Effects of intensive versus twice-weekly sessions

    Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology

    (2003)
  • AbramowitzJ.S. et al.

    Treatment compliance and outcome in obsessive-compulsive disorder

    Behavior Modification

    (2002)
  • J.S. Abramowitz et al.

    Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder in Adults

    (2014)
  • Bandarian BaloochS. et al.

    Extinction treatment in multiple contexts attenuates ABC renewal in humans

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (2012)
  • BarlowD.H. et al.

    Mastery of your anxiety and panic: Workbook

    (2007)
  • BjorkR.A.

    Memory and metamemory considerations in the training of human beings

  • BjorkR.A. et al.

    A new theory of disuse and an old theory of stimulus fluctuation

  • Cited by (88)

    • Obsessive-compulsive disorder and body dysmorphic disorder: Pediatric manifestation and treatment

      2023, Handbook of Lifespan Cognitive Behavioral Therapy: Childhood, Adolescence, Pregnancy, Adulthood, and Aging
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text