Elsevier

Clinical Psychology Review

Volume 31, Issue 6, August 2011, Pages 1004-1021
Clinical Psychology Review

The role of working memory in compulsive checking and OCD: A systematic classification of 58 experimental findings

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2011.06.004Get rights and content

Abstract

Despite the large body of research regarding the role of memory in OCD, the results are described as mixed at best (Hermans et al., 2008). For example, inconsistent findings have been reported with respect to basic capacity, intact verbal, and generally affected visuospatial memory. We suggest that this is due to the traditional pursuit of OCD memory impairment as one of the general capacity and/or domain specificity (visuospatial vs. verbal). In contrast, we conclude from our experiments (i.e., Harkin & Kessler, 2009, 2011; Harkin, Rutherford, & Kessler, 2011) and recent literature (e.g., Greisberg & McKay, 2003) that OCD memory impairment is secondary to executive dysfunction, and more specifically we identify three common factors (EBL: Executive-functioning efficiency, Binding complexity, and memory Load) that we generalize to 58 experimental findings from 46 OCD memory studies. As a result we explain otherwise inconsistent research – e.g., intact vs. deficient verbal memory – that are difficult to reconcile within a capacity or domain specific perspective. We conclude by discussing the relationship between our account and others', which in most cases is complementary rather than contradictory.

Highlights

► We provide a novel system to explain otherwise contradictory OCD memory research. ► We extend the recent executive basis of OCD memory impairment argument. ► This includes Executive-Functioning, Binding Complexity and Memory Load. ► Therefore, OCD memory impairment is not primarily an issue of capacity and/or domain. ► We then compare the EBL system to other models of memory impairment in OCD.

Introduction

Despite a large body of research the evidence for memory impairments in OCD is described as mixed at best (Hermans, Engelen, Grouwels, Joos, Lemmens, & Pieters, 2008). For example, there are inconsistent findings regarding a general mnestic deficit (e.g. Tallis, 1997 vs. MacDonald et al., 1997, McNally and Kohlbeck, 1993), verbal memory (e.g., intact: Henseler, Gruber, Kraft, Krick, Reith, & Falkai, 2008 vs. deficit: Tuna, Tekcan, & Topcuoglu, 2005) and generally affected visuospatial memory (Hermans et al., 2008, Mataix-Cols et al., 1999, Muller and Roberts, 2005). We attribute this to the traditional pursuit of OCD memory impairment as one of the general capacity and/or domain specific deficits (visuospatial vs. verbal).

In contrast, a body of research indicates a more subtle relationship, with memory impairments secondary to executive dysfunction (Greisberg & McKay, 2003). If a memory task taps into a dysfunctional component of executive functioning, memory impairment will follow (see Table 1). In this understanding, it is executive deficits in conjunction with task requirements that differentiate memory functioning in OCD from controls (Olley, Malhi, & Sachdev, 2007). This review aims at providing a more precise level of explanation based on Baddeley's extended working memory model (2000) that offers an optimal platform for relating executive functions to representations in memory.

Section snippets

A working-memory explanation

Baddeley's original model (1986) included a central executive, phonological loop and visuospatial sketchpad and was deemed separate from long-term memory (LTM). While this simple model explained a range of data (e.g., phonological similarity, word-length effect), it could not account for all experimental phenomena. For example, the visuospatial sketchpad, a capacity limit of 4 units was observed for the maintenance of individual features (colors or orientations) as well as for integrated

Empirical evidence from subclinical checkers

With these points in mind, our recent experiments (Harkin and Kessler, 2009, Harkin and Kessler, 2011, Harkin et al., 2011) set out to: (1) engage the EB using stimuli that required multimodal conjunctions between various object features and spatial locations and to (2) hamper EB functionality by confronting high and low checkers with misleading/irresolvable information during the WM retention interval. In Harkin and Kessler (2009), we employed this novel paradigm for the first time. We

The EBL (Executive-Functioning, Binding Complexity, Memory Load) classification system

Our synthesis so far leads us to conclude that checkers' memory impairment results from a complex interaction between (1) executive dysfunction in encoding organization, multimodal integration, selective attention (inhibition), maintenance control, and set-shifting and (2) the task components of load (e.g., high load, requiring chunking), multimodality (e.g., location + identity + color), distraction (e.g., dual task paradigm), retrieval dimension (e.g., location), and stimulus salience (e.g.

Applying the EBL classification system to 58 experimental findings

Fig. 1 explains where we do and do not expect to observe OCD memory impairments relative to controls; this we suggest is influenced by the degree of executive function efficiency (E), binding complexity (B) and memory load (L) within any given neuropsychological task. First, we do not expect memory performance to differ between OCD patients and controls for tasks that are low in executive demand, binding and load (see: white region in top-left quadrant of Fig. 1). Second, likelihood for

Comparison of EBL classification system to other models in the OCD literature

The EBL classification system allowed us to explain, in a unified manner, how executive impairments observed in OCD/checking tend to impair memory when the episodic buffer is extensively relied upon. However, we are aware that our EBL classification system is primarily cognitive in nature, which poses the question: How does it relate to alternative and more phenomenogical explanations of OCD symptoms in general and of memory impairments in particular?

Salkovskis (1999) provided one of the most

Limitations of the EBL classification system

We highlight the following limitations to the EBL classification system. First, it is a good fit for OCD patients with prominent checking cognitions/behaviors, but appears not to describe symptoms such as cleaning or hoarding. We propose that if the EBL factors are sufficiently stressed (as discussed above) then memory impairment could be observed in symptoms other than checking. However, we do concur that due to the specific impairments (i.e., inhibition; Omori et al., 2007) and cognitive

Conclusions

This review reconciles inconsistent findings as to memory deficits in OCD by suggesting that the classic view in terms of modality-specific (verbal vs. visuospatial) deficits and/or general capacity issues might not be the optimal way of conceiving of the problem, while we propose to follow and extend the more recent argument that OCD memory impairments are secondary to executive dysfunction. Using our research as a basis, we argue that memory impairments occur when: (1) a task taps into

References (110)

  • D. Hermans et al.

    Cognitive confidence in obsessive–compulsive disorder: Distrusting perception, attention and memory

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (2008)
  • M. Irak et al.

    Attention in sub-clinical obsessive–compulsive checkers

    Journal of Anxiety Disorders

    (2009)
  • J.H. Jang et al.

    Nonverbal memory and organizational dysfunctions are related with distinct symptom dimensions in obsessive–compulsive disorder

    Psychiatry Research

    (2010)
  • M.S. Kim et al.

    Neuropsychological profile in patients with obsessive–compulsive disorder over a period of 4-month treatment

    Journal of Psychiatric Research

    (2002)
  • A.K. Kuelz et al.

    Neuropsychological performance in obsessive–compulsive disorder: A critical review

    Biological Psychology

    (2004)
  • P.A. MacDonald et al.

    Memory and confidence in memory judgements among individuals with obsessive compulsive disorder and non-clinical controls

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (1997)
  • D. Mataix-Cols et al.

    Neuropsychological functioning in a subclinical obsessive–compulsive sample

    Biological Psychiatry

    (1999)
  • R.J. McNally et al.

    Reality monitoring in obsessive–compulsive disorder

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (1993)
  • S. Moritz et al.

    Words may not be enough! No increased emotional Stroop effect in obsessive–compulsive disorder

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (2008)
  • J. Muller et al.

    Memory and attention in obsessive–compulsive disorder: A review

    Journal of Anxiety Disorders

    (2005)
  • A. Olley et al.

    Memory and executive functioning in obsessive–compulsive disorder: A selective review

    Journal of Affective Disorders

    (2007)
  • I.M. Omori et al.

    The differential impact of executive attention dysfunction on episodic memory in obsessive–compulsive disorder patients with checking symptoms vs. those with washing symptoms

    Journal of Psychiatric Research

    (2007)
  • R. Penades et al.

    Executive function and nonverbal memory in obsessive–compulsive disorder

    Psychiatry Research

    (2005)
  • M. Petrides et al.

    Deficits on Subject-Ordered Tasks after Frontal-Lobe and Temporal-Lobe Lesions in Man

    Neuropsychologia

    (1982)
  • R. Purcell et al.

    Cognitive deficits in obsessive–compulsive disorder on tests of frontal–striatal function

    Biological Psychiatry

    (1998)
  • S. Rachman

    A cognitive theory of compulsive checking

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (2002)
  • S. Rachman et al.

    Perceived responsibility: Structure and significance

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (1995)
  • A.S. Radomsky et al.

    Don't even think about checking: Mental checking causes memory distrust

    Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry

    (2010)
  • F. Rampacher et al.

    Evidence for specific cognitive deficits in visual information processing in patients with OCD compared to patients with unipolar depression

    Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry

    (2010)
  • N.P. Rao et al.

    Are neuropsychological deficits trait markers in OCD?

    Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry

    (2008)
  • R.M. Roth et al.

    Organizational strategy use in obsessive–compulsive disorder

    Psychiatry Research

    (2004)
  • C.S. Rubenstein et al.

    Memory in sub-clinical obsessive–compulsive checkers

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (1993)
  • P.M. Salkovskis

    Understanding and treating obsessive–compulsive disorder

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (1999)
  • C.R. Savage et al.

    Organizational strategies mediate nonverbal memory impairment in obsessive–compulsive disorder

    Biological Psychiatry

    (1999)
  • K.J. Sher et al.

    Cognitive dysfunction in compulsive checkers — Further explorations

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (1984)
  • H.B. Simpson et al.

    Are there reliable neuropsychological deficits in obsessive–compulsive disorder?

    Journal of Psychiatric Research

    (2006)
  • F. Tallis et al.

    Obsessive compulsive disorder, checking, and non-verbal memory: A neuropsychological investigation

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (1999)
  • D.S. Thordarson et al.

    The Vancouver Obsessional Compulsive Inventory (VOCI)

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (2004)
  • A. Treisman

    The binding problem

    Current Opinion in Neurobiology

    (1996)
  • R.J. Allen et al.

    Is the binding of visual features in working memory resource-demanding?

    Journal of Experimental Psychology. General

    (2006)
  • A. Baddeley

    Working memory

    (1986)
  • A. Baddeley et al.

    Sentence comprehension and phonological memory — Some neuropsychological evidence

    Attention and Performance

    (1987)
  • S. Bannon et al.

    Executive functions in obsessive–compulsive disorder: State or trait deficits?

    The Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry

    (2006)
  • A.L. Benton

    The Revised Visual Retention Test

    (1974)
  • A. Bohne et al.

    Visuospatial abilities, memory, and executive functioning in trichotillomania and obsessive–compulsive disorder

    Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology

    (2005)
  • M. Boldrini et al.

    Selective cognitive deficits in obsessive–compulsive disorder compared to panic disorder with agoraphobia

    Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica

    (2005)
  • A.R. Cabrera et al.

    Missing the forest for the trees? Deficient memory for linguistic gist in obsessive–compulsive disorder

    Psychological Medicine

    (2001)
  • K.R. Cha et al.

    Nonverbal memory dysfunction in obsessive–compulsive disorder patients with checking compulsions

    Depression and Anxiety

    (2008)
  • S.R. Chamberlain et al.

    Strategy implementation in obsessive–compulsive disorder and trichotillomania

    Psychological Medicine

    (2006)
  • S.J. Chiulli et al.

    Impact of age on drawing the Rey–Osterrieth Figure

    The Clinical Neuropsychologist

    (1995)
  • Cited by (51)

    • Habitual or hyper-controlled behavior: OCD symptoms and explicit sequence learning

      2022, Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry
      Citation Excerpt :

      Furthermore, the notion of a general WM deficit in OCD is a highly contested issue. Findings show inconsistent or small effect sizes, which manifest more consistently only when binding complexity is high (e.g., in 3-back task conditions; Cooperman, 2015; Harkin & Kessler, 2011). Besides, the tendency for habitual responding in OCD patients was not observed in studies that examined cognitive load.

    • The obsessive-compulsive spectrum: A network analysis

      2022, Psychiatry Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      Thus, executive functioning allows decision-making, inhibition of learned responses, creating new routines, and prioritization (Snyder, Kaiser, et al., 2015) and is also the basis of many cognitive, emotional and social skills (Lezak et al., 2011). Increasing evidence suggests that specific impairments in executive functioning are central to the development and maintenance of obsessions and compulsions (Chamberlain et al., 2008; Harkin et al., 2011; Hill, 2004; Steinglass & Walsh, 2006; Steinglass, Walsh, et al., 2006). Once compulsive and repetitive behaviors are consolidated, it is difficult to learn and develop alternative healthy behaviors (Steinglass & Walsh, 2006).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text