Metacognitive monitoring and control processes in children with autism spectrum disorder: Diminished judgement of confidence accuracy

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2016.03.003Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Children with ASD showed diminished accuracy in their judgments of confidence.

  • Metacognitive monitoring processes, but not control processes, were impaired in ASD.

  • Impaired metacognitive monitoring in ASD has important theoretical/educational implications.

Abstract

Metacognition consists of monitoring processes (the ability to accurately represent one’s own mental states) and control processes (the ability to control one’s cognitive processes effectively). Both processes play vital roles in self-regulated learning. However, currently it is unclear whether these processes are impaired in individuals with autism spectrum disorders (ASDs). This study aimed to assess metacognition in thirty-two children with ASD, and 30 IQ-/age-matched neurotypical children, using a judgment of confidence task. It was found that children with ASD showed diminished accuracy in their judgments of confidence, indicating metacognitive monitoring impairments in ASD. Children with ASD also used monitoring to influence control processes significantly less than neurotypical children, despite little evidence of impairments in overall control ability.

Introduction

Metacognition refers to an individual’s beliefs and knowledge about cognition (often referred to as metacognitive knowledge), as well as an individual’s ability to monitor and control their own cognitive processes (often referred to as metacognitive skill). According to the standard definition, metacognition involves forming meta-representations (second order representations) of one’s own mental states. Put more simply, metacognition involves “thinking about thinking” (Flavell, 2000). It is widely thought that accurate metacognitive skill is crucial for effective self-regulation of cognition and behaviour (e.g., Nelson & Narens, 1990). By accurately monitoring one’s own mental states, one can gain a degree of control over those mental states and, by extension, control the behaviour elicited by those states (e.g., Perner, 1991). For example, whilst revising for an exam, if an individual is able to accurately assess what information they know/do not know, they can employ more effective revision techniques, thus ultimately improving their memory for the exam topic.

Whilst metacognition clearly plays an important role in self-regulation, meta-representation is also thought to play an important role in enhancing other aspects of cognition. For example, Perner (e.g., Perner, 2000) has suggested that the ability to distinguish one’s thoughts from reality is a pre-requisite for episodic remembering (but see Williams, 2010). Additionally, there is evidence to suggest that such self-related processing is involved in imagining one’s own future, and is involved in the processes of episodic future thinking (see for e.g. Wheeler, Stuss, & Tulving, 1997). In this latter respect, metacognition may play a role not only in controlling current behaviour, but also future behaviour by facilitating efficient planning for one’s future.

However, the area of cognition most frequently discussed in relation to metacognition is mindreading (i.e., the ability to represent the mental states of other people; also known as “theory of mind”). There is a substantial debate that spans developmental, cognitive, and comparative psychology, as well as philosophy and cognitive science, about the relation between mindreading and metacognition. Whereas some argue that mindreading and metacognition rely on the same underlying (metarepresentational) mechanism/processing resources (e.g., Carruthers, 2009, Gopnik, 1993), henceforth termed “one mechanism” theories, others argue either that (a) metacognition and mindreading are unrelated (e.g., Nichols & Stich, 2003) or that (b) mindreading is dependent upon metacognition, but not vice versa (e.g., Goldman, 2006).

Section snippets

Metacognition, mindreading and autism spectrum disorder

One developmental disorder that is relevant to the study of all of the above issues is autism spectrum disorder (ASD). ASD is diagnosed on the basis of behavioural deficits in social-communication, and fixated interests and repetitive behaviours (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). There is substantial evidence that, at the cognitive level, individuals with ASD manifest impairments in episodic memory (see e.g., Crane and Goddard, 2008, Lind and Bowler, 2010, Lind et al., 2014, Losh and

The current study

The central aim of this study was to extend the current findings concerning metacognition in ASD, by examining both monitoring and control accuracy in children with ASD. To examine this the study employed a JOC task, during which children were asked a series of questions about recently-studied material, and were then asked to judge how certain they were that the answers they had provided were correct (providing a measure of metacognitive monitoring accuracy). Additionally, children were told

Participants

Ethical approval for this study was obtained from the University of Kent School of Psychology Research Ethics Committee. Thirty-two children with ASD and 30 neurotypical children took part in this study, after their parents had given written, informed consent. Participants in the ASD group had formal diagnoses of autistic disorder or Asperger’s disorder, according to established criteria (American Psychiatric Association, 2000, World Heath Organisation, 1993). To assess severity of ASD

Judgment of confidence task

Table 2 shows descriptive statistics and the results of independent-samples t-tests for all aspects of the experimental JOC task. Given the predicted group differences in meta-level monitoring performance on the JOC task, all p values associated with group differences on this aspect of the task are reported one-tailed.

Before reporting the main results, it is important to ensure that participants were able to use the JOC rating scale appropriately. Thus, an initial analysis of group differences

Discussion

As predicted, this study found that participants with ASD showed diminished metacognitive monitoring accuracy, as reflected by significant between-group differences in gamma scores and in the difference score between JOC ratings for correct versus incorrect answers. These results are in keeping with our predictions and the findings of Wilkinson et al. (2010), who also found that confidence judgments made by children with ASD were less accurate than those made by neurotypical children. However,

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to sincerely thank all of the participants who took part in this study. Without their support, this research would not have been possible. The authors would also like to sincerely thank several schools in Kent for their assistance with the study; The Abbey School, Simon Langton Grammar School for Boys, The Malling School, Grange Park School, Ripplevale School, and Folkestone Academy. We would also like to thank the Kent Autistic Trust (KAT) for assistance with participant

References (54)

  • P. Carruthers

    How we know our own minds: The relationship between mindreading and metacognition

    Behavioral and Brain Sciences

    (2009)
  • J.N. Constantino et al.

    Validation of a brief quantitative measure of autistic traits: Comparison of the social responsiveness scale with the autism diagnostic interview-revised

    Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

    (2003)
  • L. Crane et al.

    Episodic and semantic autobiographical memory in adults with autism spectrum disorders

    Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

    (2008)
  • L. Crane et al.

    Remembering the past and imagining the future in autism spectrum disorder

    Memory

    (2013)
  • A. Estes et al.

    Discrepancies between academic achievement and intellectual ability in higher-functioning school-aged children with autism spectrum disorder

    Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders

    (2011)
  • N. Fisher et al.

    The relationship between vocabulary, grammar, and false belief task performance in children with autistic spectrum disorders and children with moderate learning difficulties

    Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry

    (2005)
  • J.H. Flavell

    Development of children’s knowledge about the mental world

    International Journal of Behavioral Development

    (2000)
  • U. Frith et al.

    Theory of mind and self-consciousness: What is it like to be autistic?

    Mind & Language

    (1999)
  • L.S. Fuchs et al.

    Enhancing third-grade students’ mathematical problem solving with self-regulated learning strategies

    Journal of Educational Psychology

    (2003)
  • A. Goldman

    Simulating minds: The philosophy, psychology and neuroscience of mindreading

    (2006)
  • L.A. Goodman et al.

    Measures of association for cross classifications

    Journal of the American Statistical Association

    (1954)
  • A. Gopnik

    How we know our minds – The illusion of first-person knowledge of intentionality

    Behavioral and Brain Sciences

    (1993)
  • D.J. Hacker et al.

    Test prediction and performance in a classroom context

    Journal of Educational Psychology

    (2000)
  • M.K. Hartwig et al.

    General knowledge monitoring as a predictor of in-class exam performance

    British Journal of Educational Psychology

    (2012)
  • M. Hauck et al.

    Memory for faces in children with autism

    Child Neuropsychology

    (1998)
  • C.R.G. Jones et al.

    Reading and arithmetic in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders: Peaks and dips in attainment

    Neuropsychology

    (2009)
  • A. Koriat et al.

    Monitoring and control processes in the strategic regulation of memory accuracy

    Psychological Review

    (1996)
  • Cited by (50)

    • How drug cravings affect metacognitive monitoring in methamphetamine abusers

      2022, Addictive Behaviors
      Citation Excerpt :

      Meanwhile, Lee et al. (2010) believed that metacognition also plays an important role in stopping addictive behavior and predicting relapse. Metacognition refers to personal beliefs and knowledge about cognition as well as the ability to monitor and control one’s own cognitive processes (Grainger et al., 2016); metacognition can be distinguished into two basic aspects: metacognitive regulation and metacognitive knowledge. Metacognitive regulation includes a range of executive functions, such as metacognitive monitoring which refers to the process in which an individual analyzes their own mental state, then monitors and adjusts cognitive activities to achieve a predetermined goal (Son & Schwartz, 2002; Spada & Wells, 2008).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text