The social phenotype of Williams syndrome

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2012.12.006Get rights and content

Williams syndrome (WS) offers an exciting model for social neuroscience because its genetic basis is well-defined, and the unique phenotype reflects dimensions of prosocial behaviors. WS is associated with a strong drive to approach strangers, a gregarious personality, heightened social engagement yet difficult peer interactions, high nonsocial anxiety, unusual bias toward positive affect, and diminished sensitivity to fear. New neurobiological evidence points toward alterations in structure, function, and connectivity of the social brain (amygdala, fusiform face area, orbital-frontal regions). Recent genetic studies implicate gene networks in the WS region with the dysregulation of prosocial neuropeptides. The study of WS has implications for understanding human social development, and may provide insight for translating genetic and neuroendocrine evidence into treatments for disorders of social behavior.

Highlights

WS serves as an excellent model for linking genes, neural systems and social phenotype. ► The key dimensions of WS sociability include increased approachability, attention to faces, and emotional responsivity. ► Hypersociability combines with altered structure and function of the social brain in WS. ► Genes at 7q11.23 are implicated in social-affective functions. ► The WS gene deletion is linked to dysregulation of prosocial neuropeptides oxytocin (OT) and arginine vasopressin (AVP).

Introduction

Williams syndrome (WS) is a multisystem disorder [1] characterized by a distinctive social profile that holds promise for understanding the underlying neurogenetic systems that provide meaning for human social interaction. Resulting from a hemizygous deletion of ∼25 genes on chromosome 7q11.23 [2••], a unique and robust behavioral characteristic of WS is an increased social drive particularly toward strangers [3, 4], manifesting as a strength in processing social over nonsocial stimuli, engaging language, increased social gaze, and empathic, friendly, and emotional personality [2••, 5••, 6]. This profile stands against a backdrop of strikingly uneven profile of cognitive functions, with profoundly impaired visual-spatial processing [7] (Figure 1). The neuropsychiatric profile is associated with mean IQ of approximately 50–60, with a typically higher verbal than performance IQ [1, 8]. One fascinating aspect of the WS social phenotype is that, unlike for visual-spatial processing with impaired functions across the board, the haploinsufficiency resulting from the gene deletion leads to a profile characterized by intriguing dissociations, or strengths and weaknesses (e.g. overly friendly with a difficulty in making friends; socially fearless but anxious; positive affect with maladaptive behaviors). In this sense, the WS gene deletion provides a unique model system to begin to relate single/clustered genes to specific alterations at the phenotypic level, with the ultimate potential of advancing our understanding of human social behavior at multiple levels.

This review focuses on capturing the nature of the unique WS social profile by outlining its major features at the level of behavior, followed by recent advances in the study of brain and genes. While the literature on the WS social phenotype has been building up for some time, the syndrome is emerging newly into focus and gaining new interest due to a recent surge of neurobiological evidence suggesting a consistent profile of brain structure and function underlying the social profile. Subsequently, the studies have provided critical new information about the ‘social brain’, paving the way for WS to serve as a ‘prototype model’ of social functioning that will allow new insight into understanding the biological basis of aspects of human social behavior in the future. We finish by addressing entirely new directions in molecular genetics suggesting dysregulation of prosocial neuropeptides in the overly social phenotype of WS, thereby implicating the study of WS prosocial behavior to encompass neuroendocrinology for the first time.

Section snippets

Gregarious personality, affiliative drive, and compromised social relationships

Studies of children and adults with WS highlight strikingly consistent and unique patterns of behavior both at the cognitive level [6] and in terms of sociability [5••]. The social behavior in WS also appears distinct from typical uninhibited behavior [9]. Accumulating evidence utilizing an array of methodologies (questionnaires, observations, experiments, self-reports and other reports, event related potentials (ERP), and more recently psychophysiology) has revealed increased appetitive drive

Salience of faces

Given that abnormalities in the perception and responsivity to faces are primary contributors to social dysfunction, studies have begun to address the increased attention to faces in WS, and its relation to the ‘hypersocial’ phenotype. Individuals with WS exhibit a significant interest in face stimuli across development (Figure 2) [13, 20], and spend more time focused on faces than on nonsocial stimuli [27]. Subsequently, WS is characterized by a dissociation such that those with the syndrome

The emotional phenotype

Studies of basic emotion processing have revealed a markedly uneven profile in WS, with decreased recognition of negative social signals within both visual and auditory domains by such individuals (e.g. [34]), which has been hypothesized to contribute to the increased approachability and inappropriate social engagement [35]. However, this combines with a distinct attentional and processing bias toward positive social stimuli [17], and a preserved ability to process positive affect [28, 34, 36].

The social brain

While the bulk of neurobiological literature on WS indicates diffuse abnormalities in the social brain [41••], at the same time, it paints a picture of brain structure and function that closely mirrors the excessively social and emotional behavioral profile of WS. Specifically, data are beginning to suggest that socially relevant structures are disproportionately enlarged in WS [41••, 42]. Conversely, there are both structural and functional alterations in the dorsal visual processing stream [42

Genetics and hints toward new directions

Despite being a highly heritable disorder of social dysfunction, the genetic underpinnings of autism remain largely unknown [54], and thus direct genotype–phenotype correlations are highly elusive. By contrast, the well-documented hemizygous deletion of ∼25–28 genes on chromosome 7q11.23 that results in WS [2••] is present in ∼98% of diagnosed individuals. The social profiles of WS and autism characterized by hypersociability and social avoidance respectively may appear at the first glance as

Conclusions

WS is associated with a clearly defined genetic basis, combined with an unusual, distinctive social phenotype, thereby providing an attractive model for the basis of a new approach to social neuroscience. Individuals with WS exhibit consistent and unique patterns of social behavior, characterized by an overly friendly, affectionate, engaging, and socially disinhibited personality particularly toward strangers, apparent cross-culturally, and through separable channels of communication, such as

References and recommended reading

Papers of particular interest, published within the period of review, have been highlighted as:

  • • of special interest

  • •• of outstanding interest

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by Program Project Grant NICHD 033113-15 to Bellugi, Korenberg, Semendeferi, Halgren, Gage, Muotri, and Sejnowski. In addition, we are grateful for support from NINDS 22343, and The Oak Tree Philanthropic Foundation to UB. We warmly thank the Williams Syndrome Association and families for their generous cooperation. Illustrations copyright Ursula Bellugi, The Salk Institute for Biological Studies, La Jolla, CA 92037-1099.

References (61)

  • L. Sparaci et al.

    Understanding motor acts and motor intentions in Williams syndrome

    Neuropsychologia

    (2012)
  • B.W. Haas et al.

    Social brain development in Williams syndrome: the current status and directions for future research

    Front Psychol

    (2012)
  • D. Gothelf et al.

    Association between cerebral shape and social use of language in Williams syndrome

    Am J Med Genet A

    (2008)
  • G. Golarai et al.

    The fusiform face area is enlarged in Williams syndrome

    J Neurosci

    (2010)
  • P. Malenfant et al.

    Association of GTF2I in the Williams–Beuren syndrome critical region with autism spectrum disorders

    J Autism Dev Disord

    (2012)
  • H.H. Li et al.

    Induced chromosome deletions cause hypersociability and other features of Williams–Beuren syndrome in mice

    EMBO Mol Med

    (2009)
  • B.R. Pober

    Williams–Beuren syndrome

    N Engl J Med

    (2010)
  • L. Dai et al.

    Is it Williams syndrome? GTF2IRD1 implicated in visual-spatial construction and GTF2I in sociability revealed by high resolution arrays

    Am J Med Genet A

    (2009)
  • T.F. Doyle et al.

    “Everybody in the world is my friend” hypersociability in young children with Williams syndrome

    Am J Med Genet A

    (2004)
  • U. Bellugi et al.

    Towards the neural basis for hypersociability in a genetic syndrome

    Neuroreport

    (1999)
  • A. Järvinen-Pasley et al.

    Defining the social phenotype if Williams Syndrome: a model of linking gene, the brain, and cognition

    Dev Psychopathol

    (2008)
  • B.P. Klein-Tasman et al.

    Distinctive personality characteristics of 8-, 9- and 10-year olds with Williams syndrome

    Dev Neuropsychol

    (2003)
  • C.B. Mervis et al.

    The Williams syndrome cognitive profile

    Brain Cogn

    (2000)
  • Y.M. Searcy et al.

    The relationship between age and IQ in adults with Williams syndrome

    Am J Ment Retard

    (2004)
  • A. Järvinen-Pasley et al.

    Affiliative behavior in Williams syndrome: social perception and real-life social behavior

    Neuropsychologia

    (2010)
  • H. Tager-Flusberg et al.

    A componential view of theory of mind: evidence from Williams syndrome

    Cognition

    (2000)
  • D.M. Riby et al.

    Viewing it differently: social scene perception in Williams syndrome and autism

    Neuropsychologia

    (2008)
  • J. Reilly et al.

    Frog, Where are you? Narratives in children with specific language impairment, early focal brain injury and Williams Syndrome

    Brain Lang

    (2004)
  • A. Santos et al.

    Just another face in the crowd: evidence for decreased detection of angry faces in children with Williams syndrome

    Neuropsychologia

    (2010)
  • C. Zitzer-Comfort et al.

    Nature and nurture: Williams syndrome across cultures

    Dev Sci

    (2007)
  • Cited by (119)

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text