Elsevier

Cognition

Volume 170, January 2018, Pages 179-189
Cognition

Original Articles
Automatic imitation of pro- and antisocial gestures: Is implicit social behavior censored?

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2017.09.019Get rights and content

Abstract

According to social reward theories, automatic imitation can be understood as a means to obtain positive social consequences. In line with this view, it has been shown that automatic imitation is modulated by contextual variables that constrain the positive outcomes of imitation. However, this work has largely neglected that many gestures have an inherent pro- or antisocial meaning. As a result of their meaning, antisocial gestures are considered taboo and should not be used in public. In three experiments, we show that automatic imitation of symbolic gestures is modulated by the social intent of these gestures. Experiment 1 (N = 37) revealed reduced automatic imitation of antisocial compared with prosocial gestures. Experiment 2 (N = 118) and Experiment 3 (N = 118) used a social priming procedure to show that this effect was stronger in a prosocial context than in an antisocial context. These findings were supported in a within-study meta-analysis using both frequentist and Bayesian statistics. Together, our results indicate that automatic imitation is regulated by internalized social norms that act as a stop signal when inappropriate actions are triggered.

Introduction

The propensity to imitate is well documented in psychological science (Chartrand and van Baaren, 2009, Heyes, 2011). That is, there is now strong evidence that individuals unintentionally imitate the actions (Brass et al., 2000, Chartrand and Bargh, 1999, Genschow et al., 2013), postures (Scheflen, 1964), facial expressions (Dimberg, Thunberg, & Elmehed, 2000), and speech patterns (Bock, 1986) of the people they interact with. According to ideomotor theory, these imitative tendencies are the result of shared perception-action representations in the brain (Brass et al., 2000). In particular, it is assumed that the visual image of an action is part of its motor representation and therefore that observed actions trigger an automatic imitative response (Brass et al., 2000). Interestingly, imitation is known to have important social benefits in the sense that it smooths social interaction (Chartrand & Bargh, 1999), increases prosocial behavior (van Baaren, Holland, Kawakami, & van Knippenberg, 2004), and facilitates empathy (De Coster, Verschuere, Goubert, Tsakiris, & Brass, 2013). As a result, it has been suggested that automatic imitation is an inherently social phenomenon (van Baaren, Janssen, Chartrand, & Dijksterhuis, 2009) that is driven by the social profits it generates (Lakin et al., 2003, Stel et al., 2016, Wang and Hamilton, 2012). Specifically, it has been argued that operant conditioning causes individuals to associate imitative responses with social reward, which in turn leads them to use imitation, be it conscious or subconscious, as a means to facilitate social interaction (Lakin et al., 2003, Stel et al., 2016, Wang and Hamilton, 2012).

However, the potential of imitation to produce social profit is constrained by contextual bounds. As a result, social reward theories predict that imitative tendencies should be reduced when the context makes it unlikely that they will lead to positive social outcomes (Stel et al., 2016, Wang and Hamilton, 2012). For example, it is doubtful that imitation will facilitate social interaction when the imitated person is not looking at the imitator (Wang & Hamilton, 2012). From a social reward perspective, the absence of eye contact should thus result in weaker automatic imitation, which has now been confirmed across multiple studies (Forbes et al., 2016, Wang and Hamilton, 2014, Wang et al., 2011). Moreover, if there are no constraints in the environment, social reward theories predict that automatic imitation should depend on the individual’s motivation to interact with others. In support, research has reported increased imitative tendencies in situations that promote affiliation (Butler et al., 2016, Genschow and Schindler, 2016, Lakin and Chartrand, 2003, Rauchbauer et al., 2015), stimulate prosocial attitudes (Leighton et al., 2010, Wang and Hamilton, 2013), or signal threat (Grecucci et al., 2011, Rauchbauer et al., 2016, Rauchbauer et al., 2015), but decreased imitative tendencies in situations that emphasize self-focus (Hogeveen and Obhi, 2011, Spengler et al., 2010).

In sum, previous research suggests that automatic imitation is stronger when it has the potential to generate social reward (Lakin et al., 2003, Stel et al., 2016, Wang and Hamilton, 2012). However, does this mean that individuals imitate all behavior indiscriminately as long as they are motivated to affiliate and are in a context that allows affiliation? This is important because gestures in daily life are often used in a symbolic manner as a shorthand for social communication (Morris, 1994). For example, an upwards extension of the thumb can be used to demonstrate approval (i.e., thumbs up) and an upwards extension of the middle finger can be used to communicate insult (i.e., middle finger). Nevertheless, even though communicative gestures are highly prevalent in everyday life, most automatic imitation studies have instead focused on simple actions without a symbolic meaning (Brass et al., 2001, Brass et al., 2000, Catmur and Heyes, 2011, Heyes, 2011).

As an important exception, a single study looked at automatic imitation of communicative and non-communicative gestures performed by either a human or nonhuman agent (Liepelt, Prinz, & Brass, 2010). The results revealed less imitation of the nonhuman agent compared with the human agent when a communicative action was performed (e.g., peace sign) but not when a non-communicative action was performed (e.g., grasping). However, albeit interesting, this study was silent on whether imitative tendencies depend on the social message expressed by the observed gesture. If automatic imitation is driven by its social consequences, as previous research suggests (Stel et al., 2016, Wang and Hamilton, 2012), then it should be modulated by the degree to which an imitative response would be inappropriate according to the dominant social norm (Cialdini, Reno, & Kallgren, 1990). In particular, the social norm dictates that offensive gestures ought not to be used in public. As a result, if imitation would lead to the execution of a taboo gesture, then it should be inhibited to prevent norm violation.

In line with this hypothesis, research on word production has found an increase in response inhibition when participants are at risk of making a taboo error (Severens et al., 2011, Severens et al., 2012), suggesting that individuals monitor their behavior in order to inhibit the execution of actions that would otherwise violate the established norms. However, most research on social norms conducted hitherto has looked at language production (Dhooge and Hartsuiker, 2011, Severens et al., 2011, Severens et al., 2012) or intentional social conduct such as littering (Cialdini et al., 1990) and environmental conservation (Goldstein et al., 2008, Scheibehenne et al., 2016). As a result, it remains to be understood whether more spontaneous social behavior such as automatic imitation is regulated by social norms as well. In particular, if this is the case, then the mere observation of a taboo gesture should be sufficient to trigger an implicit stop signal that prevents the unintended execution of that gesture.

To examine the role of social norms in automatic imitation, the current study compared automatic imitation of pro- and antisocial gestures. More specifically, participants performed a prosocial (i.e., thumbs up) or antisocial (i.e., middle finger) gesture in response to a symbolic cue (i.e., M or D) while a hand on the screen performed either a congruent or incongruent gesture. Automatic imitation in this paradigm can be operationalized as a congruency effect with slower responses on incongruent trials than on congruent trials (Brass et al., 2000, Heyes, 2011). If automatic imitation is sensitive to social norms, then the congruency effect should be weaker when an antisocial gesture is observed than when a prosocial gesture is observed. Moreover, as semantic processes are known to be slow (Meyer et al., 2013, Özyürek, 2014), it can be expected that this difference will become larger when the delay between the presentation of the gesture and the presentation of the cue increases. The reason for this is that longer delays provide participants with more time to process the observed gesture before a response has to be formed.

Section snippets

Participants

The sampling goal of Experiment 1 was to collect data from 40 participants, similar to our previous work on automatic imitation (Cracco, De Coster, Andres, & Brass, 2015). A sample of 40 participants provided us with 80% power to detect a medium effect size of dz = 0.45 at α = 0.05. We had no strong hypothesis regarding the size of the predicted difference between the pro- and antisocial gesture, but considered a medium-sized effect to be a reasonable assumption. In line with our sampling goal,

Participants

The sampling goal of Experiment 2 was based on a recent meta-analysis in which it was found that social primes have a small effect around d = 0.30 on behavior (Weingarten et al., 2016). In particular, our goal was to collect a sample that would allow us to detect with high power an effect of social context on the difference in automatic imitation between pro- and antisocial gestures. This was formalized in an a priori power analysis, which revealed that 118 participants were needed to detect an

Participants

The preregistered sampling goal of Experiment 3 was to collect data from 120 participants, namely 60 participants from Ghent and 60 participants from Cologne. As planned, the sample from Ghent consisted of 60 participants. However, the sample from Cologne consisted of 70 participants instead. The reason why 10 more participants were added to the Cologne sample is that the response box sometimes stopped recording responses due to a technical issue.

Meta-analysis

To assess the evidence across experiments, we conducted two fixed effects meta-analyses that tested our main hypotheses, namely that automatic imitation would be weaker when an antisocial gesture was observed than when a prosocial gesture was observed (i.e., observed gesture × congruency) and that this effect would be less pronounced in an antisocial context than in a prosocial context (i.e., context × observed gesture × congruency). Furthermore, to obtain a complete overview of the evidence, we used

General discussion

A fascinating feature of social behavior is the tendency to imitate others (Chartrand and van Baaren, 2009, Heyes, 2011). Interestingly, research suggests that imitation may have an important social function in the sense that it smooths social interaction (Chartrand & Bargh, 1999) and elicits prosocial behavior (De Coster et al., 2013, van Baaren et al., 2004). Social reward theories have taken these findings as evidence for the idea that imitative behavior is driven by the social benefits it

References (76)

  • R. Liepelt et al.

    When do we simulate non-human agents? Dissociating communicative and non-communicative actions

    Cognition

    (2010)
  • G.F. Meyer et al.

    The time course of auditory-visual processing of speech and body actions: Evidence for the simultaneous activation of an extended neural network for semantic processing

    Neuropsychologia

    (2013)
  • C. Press

    Action observation and robotic agents: Learning and anthropomorphism

    Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews

    (2011)
  • B. Rauchbauer et al.

    Distinct neural processes are engaged in the modulation of mimicry by social group-membership and emotional expressions

    Cortex

    (2015)
  • S. Spengler et al.

    Minimizing motor mimicry by myself: Self-focus enhances online action-control mechanisms during motor contagion

    Consciousness and Cognition

    (2010)
  • Y. Wang et al.

    Social top-down response modulation (STORM): A model of the control of mimicry in social interaction

    Frontiers in Human Neuroscience

    (2012)
  • A.J. Wiggett et al.

    Learning associations between action and perception: Effects of incompatible training on body part and spatial priming

    Brain and Cognition

    (2011)
  • P. Bach et al.

    Focusing on body sites: The role of spatial attention in action perception

    Experimental Brain Research

    (2007)
  • J.A. Bargh et al.

    Automaticity of social behavior: Direct effects of trait construct and stereotype activation on action

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1996)
  • K.J. Bock

    Syntactic persistence in language production

    Cognitive Psychology

    (1986)
  • M.E. Bouton

    Context, time, and memory retrieval in the interference paradigms of pavlovian learning

    Psychological Bulletin

    (1993)
  • M.E. Bouton

    Context, ambiguity, and classical conditioning

    Current Directions in Psychological Science

    (1994)
  • E.E. Butler et al.

    The influence of facial signals on the automatic imitation of hand actions

    Frontiers in Psychology

    (2016)
  • B. Calvo-Merino et al.

    Action observation and acquired motor skills: An fMRI study with expert dancers

    Cerebral Cortex

    (2005)
  • C. Catmur et al.

    Time course analyses confirm independence of imitative and spatial compatibility

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance

    (2011)
  • T.L. Chartrand et al.

    The chameleon effect: The perception-behavior link and social interaction

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1999)
  • R.B. Cialdini et al.

    Social influence: Compliance and conformity

    Annual Review of Psychology

    (2004)
  • R.B. Cialdini et al.

    A focus theory of normative conduct: Recycling the concept of norms to reduce littering in public places

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1990)
  • R. Cook et al.

    Contextual modulation of mirror and countermirror sensorimotor associations

    Journal of Experimental Psychology – General

    (2012)
  • E. Cracco et al.

    Automatic imitation of multiple agents: Simultaneous or random representation?

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance

    (2017)
  • E. Cracco et al.

    Motor simulation beyond the dyad: Automatic imitation of multiple actors

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance

    (2015)
  • E. Cracco et al.

    When your error becomes my error: Anterior insula activation in response to observed errors is modulated by agency

    Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience

    (2016)
  • L. De Coster et al.

    I suffer more from your pain when you act like me: Being imitated enhances affective responses to seeing someone else in pain

    Cognitive Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience

    (2013)
  • E. Dhooge et al.

    How do speakers resist distraction?: Evidence from a taboo picture-word interference task

    Psychological Science

    (2011)
  • A. Dijksterhuis et al.

    The relation between perception and behavior or how to win a game of trivial pursuit

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1998)
  • U. Dimberg et al.

    Unconscious facial reactions to emotional facial expressions

    Psychological Science

    (2000)
  • H. Farmer et al.

    Status and power do not modulate automatic imitation of intransitive hand movements

    PLoS ONE

    (2016)
  • T. Flaisch et al.

    Emotion and the processing of symbolic gestures: An event-related brain potential study

    Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience

    (2011)
  • Cited by (0)

    This work was supported by the Research Foundation Flanders Grant FWO14/ASP/050 awarded to the first author and by a grant from the Swiss National Science. Foundation (grant number PZ00P1_168007) awarded to the second author. The stimuli, code, data, and analyses from all experiments are available at the Open Science Framework: https://osf.io/3zucm/.

    View full text