Elsevier

Cognition

Volume 115, Issue 3, June 2010, Pages 504-511
Cognition

Brief article
Behavioral distraction by auditory novelty is not only about novelty: The role of the distracter’s informational value

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2010.03.002Get rights and content

Abstract

Unexpected events often distract us. In the laboratory, novel auditory stimuli have been shown to capture attention away from a focal visual task and yield specific electrophysiological responses as well as a behavioral cost to performance. Distraction is thought to follow ineluctably from the sound’s low probability of occurrence or, put more simply, its unexpected occurrence. Our study challenges this view with respect to behavioral distraction and argues that past research failed to identify the informational value of sound as a mediator of novelty distraction. We report an experiment showing that (1) behavioral novelty distraction is only observed when the sound announces the occurrence and timing of an upcoming visual target (as is the case in all past research); (2) that no such distraction is observed for deviant sounds conveying no such information; and that (3) deviant sounds can actually facilitate performance when these, but not the standards, convey information. We conclude that behavioral novelty distraction, as observed in oddball tasks, is observed in the presence of novel sounds but only when the cognitive system can take advantage of the auditory distracters to optimize performance.

Introduction

Unexpected novel events can capture our attention in an involuntary fashion, alerting us of a potential danger but, at the same time, potentially distracting us away from a task at hand. The registration of novel events is arguably a pivotal function of our central nervous system, one of strong clinical relevance (e.g., Fischer et al., 1999) offering a fertile avenue for fundamental research about brain functioning and cognition (e.g., Pullvermüller and Shtyrov, 2006, Schröger, 1996).

Novelty distraction has been observed in electrophysiological studies using, most often, auditory stimuli (e.g., Schröger, 2005) but also visual (e.g., Barceló et al., 2006, Polich and Comerchero, 2003) and tactile stimuli (Knight, 1996, Yamaguchi and Knight, 1991a, Yamaguchi and Knight, 1991b). Novel events trigger a pattern of responses marked by specific brain waves: an automatic novelty-detection response (mismatch negativity or MMN; e.g., Näätänen and Winkler, 1999, Picton et al., 2000), followed by an involuntary orientation response (P3a; e.g., Friedman et al., 2001, Grillon et al., 1990, Woods, 1992) and, when participants are engaged in some focal task, a re-orientation negativity (RON; e.g., Berti et al., 2004, Berti and Schröger, 2001, Schröger and Wolff, 1998). Of importance, novelty also results in measurable behavioral effects such as longer RTs and reduced response accuracy (e.g., Dawson et al., 1989, Grillon et al., 1990, Schröger, 1996, Schröger and Wolff, 1998, Woodward et al., 1991; see also Hughes et al., 2005, Hughes et al., 2007, for evidence of behavioral distraction due to auditory deviants in a visual serial recall task). It is this behavioral aspect of distraction that constitutes the focus of the present study.

Novelty distraction is also observed when target and distracter are temporally decoupled and presented in different sensory modalities, as observed in the cross-modal oddball task (e.g., Andrés et al., 2006, Escera et al., 1998), testifying of the fundamental nature of this phenomenon. In that task, participants categorize visual stimuli presented in sequence (e.g., digits) based on some criteria (e.g., parity). Each visual target is preceded by a task-irrelevant sound (standard on most trials, deviant or novel on rare trials) that participants are instructed to ignore. Just as in the auditory oddball task, auditory novels elicit a pattern of three brain responses (MMN, P3a, RON) and reduce task performance.

While most past research focused on electrophysiological responses to deviant or novel sounds, recent behavioral work suggests that (1) behavioral distraction reflects the time required for attention to switch to and from the novel sound rather than the slower processing of the target (Parmentier, Elford, Escera, Andrés, & San Miguel, 2008); and that (2) attention capture is followed by an involuntary semantic analysis of the novel’s content which can in turn impact on subsequent behavior (Parmentier, 2008, Parmentier et al., in press).

Past studies posit, somewhat peremptorily, that novelty distraction results from the novel’s low probability of occurrence and its corollary unpredictable nature. It is assumed to be about novelty per se. The objective of our study is to challenge this view and argue, instead, that (1) novelty is a necessary but not sufficient condition for novelty distraction; and that (2) an additional, yet so far unreported, important factor is the extent to which sound conveys information used by the cognitive system to optimize performance.

We suggest that past cross-modal oddball studies may not provide solid evidence that novel sounds ebb participants’ behavioral performance on the basis of their novelty per se, for the role of the sound as a warning signal has not been subject to punctilious attention. This role is evidenced by the fact that responses to a standard sound are significantly faster than in a silent condition (e.g., Andrés et al., 2006, Parmentier and Andrés, 2009), for a simple reason: in all past studies, sounds always harbingered the presentation of a target and its time of occurrence, for the interval between sound and target in this paradigm was fixed (see Table 1). Distraction, if purely reflecting the rarity of the novel’s occurrence, should be observed regardless of the sounds’ information value as a warning. To date, however, no study attempted to examine this issue.

That a stimulus preceding a target by a fixed interval speeds up response time is one of the oldest phenomena studied in psychology (e.g., Wundt, 1880; see Hackley, 2009, Niemi and Näätänen, 1981, for reviews), including across modalities (Bertelson, 1967, Davis and Green, 1969). Numerous studies have shown this effect in pure blocks where a warning always announces the upcoming presentation of a target and precedes it by a fixed amount of time, but not in mixed blocks where targets do not always follow warnings or do so after a temporal interval varying from trial to trial (e.g., Näätänen, 1970, Woodrow, 1914). This type of warning informs participants of the certainty of the target’s presentation and allows the orientation their attention to a particular point in time (Hackley, 2009).

We suggest that the notion that an event’s rarity is the sole determinant of behavioral distraction in the cross-modal task may be precarious, and that the potential role of the information conveyed by auditory distracters has not received sufficient attention. To address this issue, we asked participants to categorize visual digits preceded by standard and deviant sounds in three conditions. In the Informative Condition, the sound announced the upcoming of a target after a fixed temporal interval. In the Uninformative Condition, targets followed sounds with a probability of .5 and the interval separating them varied, thereby stripping the sound of its warning value. In the Informative Deviant Condition, the standard sound did not announce whether or when a target would be presented but the novel sound did. Two sets of predictions were opposed. According to the probabilistic view of distraction, deviants should distract participants as long as they are rare, therefore predicting similar levels of distraction in all three conditions. The informational view posits that the impact of novelty may be mediated by the informational content of sound, predicting distraction in the Informative Condition, no distraction in the Uninformative Condition, and facilitation in the Informative Deviant Condition.

Section snippets

Participants

Sixty-six (54 women) undergraduates from the University of Plymouth took part in this experiment in exchange for a small honorarium. Participants were between 18 and 44 years of age (M = 20.17, SD = 4.13). All participants reported correct or corrected-to-normal vision and normal hearing.

Stimuli, design and procedure

Participants were presented with 1440 test trials each (four blocks of 360). In each trial, they categorized a visual digit (1–6) as odd or even using two arbitrary allocated keys (counterbalanced across

Results

Both accuracy and mean response latencies were examined in the critical trials common to all three conditions, that is, trials in which a visual target was presented 250 ms after the onset of the sound.

Overall, participants performed well (M = .88, SD = .06). The proportion of correct responses (indicated numerically in Fig. 1) was analyzed using a 2 (standard versus novel) × 3 (condition) mixed-design ANOVA. The main effect of condition was significant, F(2, 63) = 5.480, MSE = .007, p < .01, ηp2 = .148. No

Discussion

The results of the present study are clear-cut: the behavioral distraction yielded by novel sounds on a visual categorization task varied based on the informational value of the sound. When the sound predicted the occurrence of a target and its timing, as is the case in all past cross-modal oddball task (see Table 1), novels delayed participants’ responses relative to standards. However, when sound was stripped of its informational value, auditory novelty had no impact on performance, clearly

Acknowledgements

This work was carried out with the support of a Ramon y Cajal Fellowship (RYC-2007-00701) and a research grant (PSI-2009-08427) from the Spanish Ministry of Science and Innovation, awarded to Fabrice Parmentier.

References (70)

  • I.P. Jääskeläinen et al.

    Effects of ethanol and auditory distraction on forced choice reaction time

    Alcohol

    (1996)
  • S. Mäntysalo et al.

    The duration of a neuronal trace of an auditory stimulus as indicated by event-related potentials

    Biological Psychology

    (1987)
  • R. Näätänen

    The diminishing time-uncertainty with the lapse of time after the warning signal in reaction-time experiments with varying fore-periods

    Acta Psychologica

    (1970)
  • W. Notebaert et al.

    Post-error slowing: An orienting account

    Cognition

    (2009)
  • F.B.R. Parmentier

    Towards a cognitive model of distraction by auditory novelty: The role of involuntary attention capture and semantic processing

    Cognition

    (2008)
  • F.B.R. Parmentier et al.

    The cognitive locus of distraction by acoustic novelty in the cross-modal oddball task

    Cognition

    (2008)
  • M.D. Polo et al.

    Electrophysiological evidence of abnormal activation of the cerebral network of involuntary attention in alcoholism

    Clinical Neurophysiology

    (2003)
  • M.D. Polo et al.

    ERPs and behavioural indices of long-term preattentive and attentive deficits after closed head injury

    Neuropsychologia

    (2002)
  • H. Pratt et al.

    ‘Oddball’ event-related potentials and information processing during REM and non-REM sleep

    Clinical Neurophysiology

    (1999)
  • U. Roeber et al.

    Auditory distraction with different presentation rates: An even-related potential and behavioral study

    Clinical Neurophysiology

    (2003)
  • U. Roeber et al.

    Response repetition vs. response change modulates behavioral and electrophysiological effects of distraction

    Cognitive Brain Research

    (2005)
  • M. Sabri et al.

    Effects of sequential and temporal probability of deviant occurrence on mismatch negativity

    Cognitive Brain Research

    (2001)
  • I. San Miguel et al.

    On the functional significance of Novelty-P3: Facilitation by unexpected novel sounds

    Biological Psychology

    (2010)
  • E. Schröger et al.

    Behavioral and electrophysiological effects of task-irrelevant sound change: A new distraction paradigm

    Cognitive Brain Research

    (1998)
  • O. van der Stelt

    Auditory P300 and mismatch negativity in comatose states

    Clinical Neurophysiology

    (2008)
  • R. Van Mourik et al.

    When distraction is not distracting: A behavioral and ERP study on distraction in ADHD

    Clinical Neurophysiology

    (2007)
  • D.L. Woods

    Auditory selective attention in middleaged and elderly subjects: An event-related brain potential study

    Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology

    (1992)
  • S. Yamaguchi et al.

    P300 generation by novel somatosensory stimuli

    Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology

    (1991)
  • S. Yamaguchi et al.

    Age effects on the P300 to novel somatosensory stimuli

    Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology

    (1991)
  • K. Alho et al.

    Effects of involuntary auditory attention on visual task performance and brain activity

    Neuroreport

    (1997)
  • F. Barceló et al.

    Task switching and novelty processing activate a common neural network for cognitive control

    Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience

    (2006)
  • F. Barceló et al.

    An informational–theoretical approach to contextual processing in the human brain: Evidence from prefrontal lesions

    Cerebral Cortex

    (2007)
  • P. Bertelson

    The time course of preparation

    Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology

    (1967)
  • S. Berti

    Cognitive control after distraction: Event-related brain potentials (ERPs) dissociate between different processes of attentional allocation

    Psychophysiology

    (2008)
  • S. Berti et al.

    Bottom-up influences on working memory: Behavioral and electrophysiological distraction varies with distractor strength

    Experimental Psychology

    (2004)
  • Cited by (78)

    • The effect of target-related and target-irrelevant novel stimuli on response behaviour

      2023, Acta Psychologica
      Citation Excerpt :

      Distraction by novelty is reflected by psychophysiological measures, such as the novelty P3 event-related potential component (Courchesne et al., 1975), but also by increases in response times (RT) and error rates (e.g. Parmentier et al., 2011). However, this distracting effect by target-irrelevant novel stimuli is not always observed (Parmentier et al., 2010). When novel stimuli are target-related, they can prolong RTs as well (Krebs, Schott, Schütze, & Düzel, 2009; Exp 1), possibly due to distraction, or more likely, the result of attentional lingering to promote stimulus encoding (Kafkas & Montaldi, 2011).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text