Elsevier

Cognition

Volume 108, Issue 3, September 2008, Pages 652-661
Cognition

Why good thoughts block better ones: The mechanism of the pernicious Einstellung (set) effect

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2008.05.005Get rights and content

Abstract

The Einstellung (set) effect occurs when the first idea that comes to mind, triggered by familiar features of a problem, prevents a better solution being found. It has been shown to affect both people facing novel problems and experts within their field of expertise. We show that it works by influencing mechanisms that determine what information is attended to. Having found one solution, expert chess players reported that they were looking for a better one. But their eye movements showed that they continued to look at features of the problem related to the solution they had already thought of. The mechanism which allows the first schema activated by familiar aspects of a problem to control the subsequent direction of attention may contribute to a wide range of biases both in everyday and expert thought – from confirmation bias in hypothesis testing to the tendency of scientists to ignore results that do not fit their favoured theories.

Introduction

The difficulty lies, not in the new ideas, but in escaping from the old ones, which ramify ... into every corner of our minds. John Maynard Keynes (1936/1973; p. xxiii)

The counter-intuitive possibility that prior knowledge can have a negative effect on future performance is a theme in a range of areas of psychology that at first sight might seem unrelated. For example, in negative transfer paradigms previous experience makes it more difficult to adapt to a new setting than it would be without such experience (Besnard and Cacitti, 2005, McCloy et al., 2007, Schwartz et al., 2005, Singley and Anderson, 1989, Thorndike and Woodworth, 1901). Similarly, in the part-set cueing phenomenon people who are given a portion of previously studied/memorized material were in fact hampered by this ‘help’ when they tried to recall the remaining material compared to people who were just asked to recall the material (Basden and Basden, 1995, Roediger, 1973. Another example is offered by insight problems, first investigated by Köhler, 1925, Maier, 1930, Maier, 1931, Duncker, 1945. Although there are different theories of why these problems are so hard to solve, most agree that the difficulty lies in the initial mental representation of the problem, determined by prior knowledge, from which people are unable to escape even though it does not lead to a successful solution (Kaplan and Simon, 1990, Kershaw and Ohlsson, 2004, Knoblich et al., 1999, MacGregor et al., 2001, Ohlsson, 1992, Smith, 1995). A similar effect can be found in the motor system. Common actions become automated through frequent use and are triggered by familiar situations without conscious thought. This usually leads to efficient responses to the demands of everyday life. But when a specific context is strongly associated with a well-learnt, but now inappropriate, pattern, the familiar pattern can emerge with unintended results (Norman, 1981). These may be amusing (or embarrassing) if one dials a familiar telephone number at the wrong time (e.g., Woltz, Gardner, & Bell, 2000). But the result can be catastrophic if someone controlling a nuclear reactor executes a common but now inappropriate sequence (Reason, 1990). Finally, the way people seek information is biased by their prior knowledge. When people test a theory, they look for evidence that is consistent with what they already believe rather than objectively assessing any evidence even if it might disconfirm their previously held belief (Dawson et al., 2002, McKenzie, 2006, Nickerson, 1998, Wason, 1960, Westen et al., 2006).

An instance of the negative impact of previous knowledge is known as the Einstellung (set) effect. This occurs when the first idea that comes to mind, triggered by previous experience with similar situations, prevents alternatives being considered. If this initial idea is not the best way to solve the problem, the optimum solution may be missed. The effect was first demonstrated experimentally by Luchins (1942), who gave people a series of problems that could be solved by a fixed method which they quickly learnt. Then he gave them a problem that appeared similar to the previous ones but which could not be solved by the same method (the ‘extinction’ problem). Many said it was insoluble. The fixation of thought displayed by these people was demonstrated by a control group who were given only the extinction problem. They solved it quickly, showing that the problem was not intrinsically difficult. The experimental group failed to find the solution because the similarity of the final problem to the previous ones brought the usual (and now inappropriate) method to mind, preventing them from considering alternatives.

The Einstellung effect has been repeatedly demonstrated in the laboratory in a variety of forms using problems that do not require prior knowledge of the problem domain (e.g., Atwood and Polson, 1976, Chen and Mo, 2004, Delaney et al., 2004, Lippman, 1996, Lovett and Anderson, 1996). These studies show that mental set can be induced by a small number of similar problems in people who have never experienced the task before. It can also be used to demonstrate the mechanisms behind the mistakes made by experts. Experts rarely make mistakes but when they do, it is usually because they think the situation is a familiar one and apply their usual, but now inappropriate, methods to find a solution (Reason, 1990, Singley and Anderson, 1989). For example, most errors that doctors make are not connected to their inadequate medical knowledge but rather to the tendency to form opinions quickly based on previous experience. Once the initial diagnosis is formed, it guides doctors in the search of supporting evidence which in turn brings dangers of missing important aspects unrelated to the initial diagnosis (Croskerry, 2003, Graber et al., 2005, Groopman, 2007, Kassirer and Kopelman, 1989). Such errors by experts with real life problems (where the errors are induced by knowledge of a well-learned procedure in long-term memory) resemble errors induced by Einstellung problems in the laboratory (where the mind set is induced by short-term memory of a newly learned procedure).

Despite the potential importance of the Einstellung effect for understanding why experts sometimes fail to find the optimum solution to a problem within their area of expertise, there are few studies investigating the effect with experts (for similar research on how experts can fail outside their domain, see Wiley, 1998, Frensch and Sternberg, 1989, Hecht and Proffitt, 1995). One set of studies that has looked at this is Saariluoma, 1990, Saariluoma, 1992 demonstration that skilful chess players can fall victim to the Einstellung effect. The players tried to solve a number of chess problems where a familiar motif led to the solution. They were then presented with a problem that had two solutions. One was the familiar solution they had used to solve the previous problem, but was sub-optimal; the other was less familiar but optimal. Most players failed to find the optimal solution in the presence of the familiar solution. Saariluoma concluded that chess players can suffer from the Einstellung effect.

Bilalić, McLeod, and Gobet (2008) extended Saariluoma’s observations, confirming that expert chess players can experience the Einstellung effect, and showing that it can be quantified. They showed expert players (Candidate Masters, Masters, and International Masters1) a number of problems such as the one shown in Fig. 1a, and asked them to find the shortest way for White to achieve checkmate. There were two solutions, one a well-known solution (smothered mate) taking five moves and the other (the optimal solution) a less familiar one taking three moves. The players who found the familiar solution but failed to find the optimal one were then shown a similar problem where the familiar solution had been disabled, leaving only the optimal one (shown in Fig. 1b). All the experts found the shorter solution in the 1-solution problem, showing that they were capable of finding it when not distracted by the familiar one. To quantify the impact which the familiar solution had on the performance of expert players in the 2-solution problem, Bilalić et al. (2008) exploited one of the advantages of chess as a domain for studying problem solving – the presence of an interval scale for measurement of skill (see Footnote 1).

Similarly to the studies on transfer, Bilalić et al. (2008) used a control group of weaker players (Class A, Class B, and Class C) and showed them the 1-solution problem only. The Einstellung effect was quantified by seeing how much weaker a player had to be, when only the optimal solution was present, to show comparable performance to that of a better player when the distracting effect of the familiar solution was present (for similar measures of transfer see the first chapter of Singley & Anderson, 1989). The performance of the International Masters (5 SDs above average) on the 2-solution problem was comparable to that of the Class A players (2 SDs above average) on the 1-solution problem, the performance of the Masters (4 SDs above average) was comparable to that of the Class B players (1 SD above average) and the performance of the Candidate Masters (3SDs above average) was the same as the Class C players (average). Across a range of skill levels, the presence of a familiar solution that first came to mind reduced the problem solving performance of the experts to that of players about three standard deviations lower in skill. Similar results were obtained using different problems and the more naturalistic instruction to find the best move (Bilalić et al., 2008). Three standard deviations is a gulf in skill level. The chance of a player being beaten by one 600 Elo points lower is close to zero. And yet the Einstellung effect temporarily reduced the problem solving ability of the experts to that of the less skilled players. It is a very powerful effect.

Bilalić et al. (2008) also found that the Einstellung effect could not be demonstrated with Grand Masters (players more than five SDs above mean chess skill) with the problems shown in Fig. 1 because they all found the optimal solution to the 2-solution problem. But Grandmasters did show the Einstellung effect in problems where the optimal solution was more difficult to find. Similarly, with problems where the less obvious solution was easier to find than in this one, the Einstellung effect could no longer be demonstrated with Candidate Masters because they all found the optimal solution to the 2-solution problem. But with these easier problems the effect could now be measured with weaker players (e.g., Class A) as the necessary control group of even weaker players (i.e., weaker than Class C) started to find the optimal solution in the 1-solution problem. Thus the demonstration of the Einstellung effect depends on an interaction between the skill of the individual and the difficulty of the less obvious solution. With harder problems it can be demonstrated with stronger players, with easier problems it can be demonstrated with weaker players. The ability to resist the Einstellung effect and find the optimal solution in any given problem is greater the more skilful the player. But the effect can still be shown, with a problem of appropriate difficulty, whatever the level of expertise of the individual player.

We show, by measuring players’ eye movements, that the mechanism by which the first idea prevents a better idea coming to mind can be demonstrated. Crucially, we find that players believed that they were actively searching for better solutions when in fact they continued to look at aspects of the problem related to the first idea they considered. This is why the Einstellung effect is pernicious – people do not realize that it is influencing their thoughts.

Section snippets

Experiment 1

Why did many of the players fail to find the optimal solution to the 2-solution problem (Fig. 1a) in Bilalić et al. (2008)? Is it possible that the players were so blinded by the familiar solution that they did not notice at all the critical features necessary to find the better one? Or is it plausible that they noticed the critical features but could not connect them with the optimal solution? In the previous experiments we used verbal protocols to glean insights into the mechanisms involved

Experiment 2

Although they showed that the effect is very powerful, Bilalić et al. (2008) did not fully replicate Luchins’ original results (1942) where many participants could not find the solution to the 1-solution problem. It is not surprising that expert chess players, three and more standard deviations above the skill level of average players, were able to find a three-move mate when the distracting effect of the familiar solution was removed. The question remains, however, whether the players were

General discussion

In previous research (Bilalić et al., 2008), we showed that the Einstellung effect, the blocking of new ideas by the first one that comes to mind, reduced the problem solving ability of chess experts to that of players about three standard deviations below them in skill. In the present paper we show that the origin of the effect was that players continued to look at the squares related to the first idea they had, even though they reported that they were looking for alternative solutions.

The eye

Acknowledgments

We thank Kim Plunkett for providing the equipment for the eye movement experiment; Vanja Vučetić for help with data analysis; Jill O’Reilly, Dennis Rünger, Robert Gaschler, Guillermo Campitelli, Géza Harsányi, Stellan Ohlsson, Neil Charness, and two anonymous reviewers for their comments on earlier drafts of the paper. We are especially grateful to all chess players for participating in the experiments. Supported by an ESRC Postdoctoral Fellowship, Oxford University Clarendon Bursary, and

References (83)

  • J.P. Kassirer et al.

    Cognitive errors in diagnosis: Instantiation, classification, and consequences

    American Journal of Medicine

    (1989)
  • G. Keren

    On the importance of identifying the correct ‘problem space’

    Cognition

    (1984)
  • M.C. Lovett et al.

    History of success and current context in problem solving: Combined influences on operator selection

    Cognitive Psychology

    (1996)
  • C.M. Anderson et al.

    Remembering can cause forgetting: Retrieval dynamics in long-term memory

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition

    (1994)
  • F. Bacon

    Novum organum

  • J. Baron

    Thinking and deciding

    (1994)
  • D.R. Basden et al.

    Some tests of the strategy disruption interpretation of part-list cuing inhibition

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition

    (1995)
  • E.M. Bowden et al.

    Getting the right idea: Semantic activation in the right hemisphere may help solve insight problems

    Psychological Science

    (1998)
  • N. Charness et al.

    The perceptual aspect of skilled performance in chess: Evidence from eye movements

    Memory & Cognition

    (2001)
  • Z. Chen et al.

    Schema induction in problem solving: A multidimensional analysis

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition

    (2004)
  • P. Croskerry

    The importance of cognitive errors in diagnosis and strategies to minimize them

    Academic Medicine

    (2003)
  • E. Dawson et al.

    Motivated reasoning and performance on the Wason selection task

    Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin

    (2002)
  • A.D. De Groot et al.

    Perception and memory in chess

    (1996)
  • P.F. Delaney et al.

    Immediate and sustained effects of planning in a problem-solving task

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition

    (2004)
  • K. Drinkwater et al.

    Effects of part-set cuing on experienced and novice chess players’ reconstruction of a typical chess midgame position

    Perceptual & Motor Skills

    (2006)
  • K. Duncker

    On problem-solving

    Psychological Monographs

    (1945)
  • A.E. Elo

    The rating of chess players, past and present

    (1978)
  • K.A. Ericsson et al.

    Protocol analysis: Verbal reports as data (rev. Ed.)

    (1993)
  • P.A. Frensch et al.

    Expertise and intelligent thinking: When is it worse to know better?

  • J. Friedrich

    Primary error detection and minimization (PEDMIN) strategies in social cognition: A reinterpretation of confirmation bias phenomena

    Psychological Review

    (1993)
  • H. Gardner

    Changing minds

    (2004)
  • S.J. Gould

    The mismeasure of man

    (1996)
  • M.L. Graber et al.

    Diagnostic error in internal medicine

    Archives of Internal Medicine

    (2005)
  • E.R. Grant et al.

    Eye movements and problem solving: Guiding attention guides thought

    Psychological Science

    (2003)
  • J. Groopman

    How doctors think

    (2007)
  • H. Hecht et al.

    The price of expertise: Effects of experience on the water-level task

    Psychological Science

    (1995)
  • T.L. Hodgson et al.

    The strategic control of gaze direction in the Tower of London task

    Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience

    (2000)
  • G. Jones

    Testing two cognitive theories of insight

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition

    (2003)
  • A.M. Just et al.

    Eye fixations and cognitive processes

    Cognitive Psychology

    (1976)
  • D.E. Kanouse

    Language, labeling, and attribution

  • T.C. Kershaw et al.

    Multiple causes of difficulty in insight: The case of the nine-dot problem

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition

    (2004)
  • Cited by (164)

    • The entrenchment effect: Why people persist with less-preferred behaviors

      2023, Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes
    • Auditor industry range and audit quality

      2023, Journal of Accounting and Economics
    • Creative problem solving

      2023, Handbook of Organizational Creativity: Individual and Group Level Influences, Second Edition
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text