Dual-task repetition alters event-related brain potentials and task performance

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2011.10.001Get rights and content

Abstract

Objective

We examined the modulation of event-related brain potentials (ERPs) and the accuracy of sensori-motor coordination on short-term repetition of the concurrent performance of a somatosensory discrimination (oddball) task and a visuo-motor tracking task.

Methods

The subjects concurrently performed visuomotor tracking and somatosensory oddball tasks. In the dual-task condition, the subjects performed the visuomotor tracking and somatosensory oddball tasks concurrently for about an hour. In the oddball-only condition, they performed just the oddball task for the same period.

Results

Tracking performance improved with task repetition. The amplitude of the P300 elicited by somatosensory stimulation in the oddball-only condition decreased significantly with task repetition, whereas in the dual-task condition, it showed a complex pattern of change. The earlier responses were decreased in amplitude in the dual-task condition compared to the oddball-only condition, and gradually decreased with task repetition in both conditions.

Conclusions

Dynamic changes in ERPs and task performance with dual-task repetition support the idea that dual-task repetition produces changes in resource allocation following the automation of stimulus processing in addition to so-called habituation.

Significance

This study also provides evidence for use of ERP amplitudes as physiological indices of functionally different types of resources.

Highlights

► The amplitude of the P300 showed a complex pattern of change with dual-task repetition, which is different in the time course from so-called habituation. ► This may be associated with resource allocation in addition to so-called habituation. ► The different patterns among P100, N140, P300 and behavioral performance support the previous suggestion that they are associated with functionally different types of resources.

Introduction

Everyday life often requires the performance of two or more tasks simultaneously. A good example comes from driving. A beginner driver can have difficulty performing multiple tasks, e.g., monitoring the road through the windshield, turning the steering wheel, manipulating the gas pedal and brake, and looking at the speedometer. However, a skilled driver can perform multiple sensori-motor tasks smoothly. How then do people learn to perform these complex multiple tasks?

Habituation is one reflection of sensory-motor learning. The P300 of event-related brain potentials (ERPs) (Donchin and Coles, 1988, Katayama and Polich, 1999, Polich, 2007) decreases in amplitude with task or stimulus repetition, i.e., habituation, because the updating of the stimulus environment neural model becomes automated (Polich and McIsaac, 1994, Ravden and Polich, 1998, Ravden and Polich, 1999, Romero and Polich, 1996). The amplitude of the earlier response, the N1, elicited by auditory, visual and somatosensory stimulation is also decreased with stimulus repetition (Budd et al., 1998, Callaway, 1973, Kida et al., 2004c, Kida et al., 2006b, Ritter et al., 1968, Tomberg et al., 1989). According to the orienting response theory (Sokolov, 1960, Sokolov, 1963), habituation reflects the establishment or updating of a neuronal model or template of a stimulus following repeated exposure. In this context, ERP response habituation refers to a decrement in response amplitude resulting from a loss of novelty associated with the building of a neuronal model following repetition of a stimulus (Budd et al., 1998). According to this theory, the development of automatic processing with task repetition in an effortful task leads to an increased amount of resources available for the other task being performed if the resources have a modality-nonspecific feature. These habituation experiments have been, however, carried out mainly in a single-task situation.

Dual-task studies have found that the amplitude of the P300 elicited by target stimuli in an oddball task decreased during the concurrent performance of the other task, and showed a further decrease when the difficulty of the latter task increased. These results have demonstrated that the amplitude of the P300 reflects the amount of perceptual-central resources allocated to a given task (Isreal et al., 1980a, Isreal et al., 1980b, Isreal et al., 1980c, Kida et al., 2004a, Kramer et al., 1983, Kramer et al., 1985, Murray and Janelle, 2007, Sirevaag et al., 1989, Wickens et al., 1983, Wickens, 1980, Wickens, 1991). In general, the P300 is a modality-nonspecific component which can be elicited by stimulation of any sense, especially by infrequent target stimuli in a sequence of frequent standard stimuli. This nonspecific feature of the P300 suggested to us that the resource manifested by the P300 amplitude also shows modality-nonspecific features. Using a dual-task situation across different senses where a visuomotor tracking task and a somatosensory oddball task were performed simultaneously, a previous ERP study found that the amplitude of P300 elicited by somatosensory stimulation reflects modality-nonspecific resources (Kida et al., 2004a). A dual-task experiment using a visual memory search task and a pursuit step tracking task have reported that reductions in the slope of the memory set function with task repetition occurred earlier for P300 latency than for reaction time (RT), assuming that the stimulus evaluation process became automated more rapidly than the response selection components of memory search (Kramer et al., 1991). However, there have been no additional evidence for ERP changes with dual-task repetition, and thus it remains an open question how the ERPs are modulated by dual-task repetition.

In the present study, we examined the effect of repetition of dual task on the somatosensory ERPs (P100, N140 and P300) and task performance in a dual-task situation where we have previously examined a relationship between resource allocation and somatosensory ERPs (Kida et al., 2004a). Two conditions were conducted on separate days. In the dual-task condition, a visuomotor force-tracking task and a somatosensory oddball task were performed concurrently. In the oddball-only condition, the somatosensory oddball task was just performed. Tracking accuracy in the visuo-motor tracking task, and ERPs and reaction time in the oddball task were measured.

Section snippets

Subjects

Recordings were obtained from 10 healthy subjects (2 females and 8 males), aged 22–29 years. The subjects were seated, and their left and right hands and forearms were attached comfortably to the arm of the chair. All participants provided informed consent.

Somatosensory stimuli

Electrical stimuli of 0.2 ms duration were randomly presented to the first and third digits of the right hand through ring electrodes attached to the first (anode) and second (cathode) interphalangeal spaces. In half of the subjects, frequent

Performance

Fig. 1 shows performance data for the visuomotor tracking task and somatosensory oddball task. Tracking error decreased with task repetition (F(9,81) = 2.3, P < 0.05), suggesting that performance was improved by task repetition. Significant improvement began in the 6th block (Fig. 1B). RT (Fig. 1C) did not show any significant change with task repetition, but was delayed in the dual-task condition compared to the oddball-only condition (F(1,9) = 36.3, P < 0.001). Variance of RT (Fig. 1D) was greater in

Task performance

Tracking performance improved with task repetition, indicating the processing related to the tracking task to be somewhat automated over trials. Significant improvement began at the 6th block. RT in the oddball task did not change significantly with repetition. This dissociation between RT and tracking performance may be due to a difference in task difficulty between the tracking and oddball tasks. The tracking task is more demanding than the oddball task, and thus might produce a greater

Conclusion

The present study demonstrated that the attenuation of P300 amplitude with repetition in the oddball-only condition was so-called habituation whereas the complex change in P300 amplitude in the dual-task condition involved not only habituation, but also the allocation of modality-nonspecific resources released from a qualitatively-different visuomotor tracking task. The different patterns among P100, N140, P300 and behavioral performance support the previous suggestion that they are associated

References (54)

  • T. Kida et al.

    Resource allocation and somatosensory P300 amplitude during dual task: effects of tracking speed and predictability of tracking direction

    Clin Neurophysiol

    (2004)
  • T. Kida et al.

    Differential modulation of temporal and frontal components of the somatosensory N140 and the effect of interstimulus interval in a selective attention task

    Brain Res Cogn Brain Res

    (2004)
  • T. Kida et al.

    Passive enhancement of the somatosensory P100 and N140 in an active attention task using deviant alone condition

    Clin Neurophysiol

    (2004)
  • T. Kida et al.

    Centrifugal regulation of human cortical responses to a task-relevant somatosensory signal triggering voluntary movement

    NeuroImage

    (2006)
  • A. Kok

    Event-related-potential (ERP) reflections of mental resources: a review and synthesis

    Biol Psychol

    (1997)
  • J. Polich

    Updating P300: an integrative theory of P3a and P3b

    Clin Neurophysiol

    (2007)
  • J. Polich et al.

    Comparison of auditory P300 habituation from active and passive conditions

    Int J Psychophysiol

    (1994)
  • D. Ravden et al.

    Habituation of P300 from visual stimuli

    Int J Psychophysiol

    (1998)
  • D. Ravden et al.

    On P300 measurement stability: habituation, intra-trial block variation, and ultradian rhythms

    Biol psychol

    (1999)
  • W. Ritter et al.

    Orienting and habituation to auditory stimuli: a study of short term changes in average evoked responses

    Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol

    (1968)
  • R. Romero et al.

    P3(00) habituation from auditory and visual stimuli

    Physiology & Behavior

    (1996)
  • E.J. Sirevaag et al.

    Resource reciprocity: an event-related brain potentials analysis

    Acta Psychologica

    (1989)
  • C. Tomberg et al.

    Mapping somatosensory evoked potentials to finger stimulation at intervals of 450 to 4000 msec and the issue of habituation when assessing early cognitive components

    Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol

    (1989)
  • J. Van Velzen et al.

    Shifts of attention in the early blind: an erp study of attentional control processes in the absence of visual spatial information

    Neuropsychologia

    (2006)
  • J.E. Desmedt et al.

    Differential enhancement of early and late components of the cerebral somatosensory evoked potentials during forced-paced cognitive tasks in man

    J Physiol

    (1977)
  • E. Donchin et al.

    Is the P300 component a manifestation of context updating?

    Brain Behav Sci

    (1988)
  • M. Eimer et al.

    Cross-modal links in endogenous spatial attention are mediated by common external locations: evidence from event-related brain potentials

    Exp Brain Res Experimentelle Hirnforschung

    (2001)
  • Cited by (25)

    • Adaptive flexibility of the within-hand attentional gradient in touch: An MEG study

      2018, NeuroImage
      Citation Excerpt :

      Some studies have also suggested that different types of resources are represented in different ERP components. Early ERP components have been associated with perceptual resources (Kida et al., 2012a; b; Kida et al., 2004a; Kok, 1997) in the framework of multiple resources including perceptual, central, and response resources as a stage dimension (Wickens, 1991; Wickens and McCarley, 2008). The attentional increase in the SIIc response may, thus, be associated with the perceptual resource.

    • Cognitive performance under motor demands – On the influence of task difficulty and postural control

      2018, Brain Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      Originally described by Ritter and Vaughan (1969), the task comprises of the presentation of sequences of repetitive stimuli, which are infrequently interrupted by a deviant stimulus. Within dual-task studies, the oddball-task (as secondary task) has been used in combination with visual tracking tasks (Wickens et al., 1977; Isreal et al., 1980a), simulator tasks (Kramer et al., 1987), or visuomotor force-tracking tasks (Kida et al., 2012). Results showed that the amplitude of the P3/300, which is triggered by deviating stimuli, depends on the difficulty of the primary task only under the premise that the difficulty is manipulated within the perceptual-central domain.

    • The effect of badminton training on the ability of same-domain action anticipation for adult novices: Evidence from behavior and ERPs

      2017, Neuroscience Letters
      Citation Excerpt :

      The P3 amplitude for the control group reduced at post-test compared to pre-test. Many researchers suggested that the reduction in P3 amplitude reflected fewer resources allocated to target processing [10,35–41]. One possible explanation for the phenomenon may be the reduction of resource allocation due to repeated tests [42,43].

    • Effects of mental workload on involuntary attention: A somatosensory ERP study

      2017, Neuropsychologia
      Citation Excerpt :

      For instance, when people are engaged in doing multi-tasks that continuously deliver visual and auditory information to them (i.e., situation of divided attention) or doing mentally demanding works based on visual and auditory modality such as flight simulation tasks that require heightened vigilance to multiple inputs, using additional visual or auditory stimuli as ERP probes could interfere with their concentration on the tasks. To resolve this concern, some studies have investigated attentional modulation of somatosensory ERP in dual tasks and demonstrated somatosensory ERP correlates of mental workload (Jones and Forster, 2013; Kida et al., 2004, 2012; Santangelo and Spence, 2007; Sugimoto and Katayama, 2013). Santangelo and Spence (2007) reported that spatial selective attention of exogenous tactile stimuli can be differently modulated as long as attentional resources are not depleted by a visually demanding task.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text