Bilingualism and socioemotional well-being

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2010.01.009Get rights and content

Abstract

Using the Early Childhood Longitudinal Study — Kindergarten Cohort (ECLS-K), this paper examines Latino children's socioemotional trajectories from kindergarten to fifth grade, paying particular attention to children's language proficiency. Results from the growth-curve analysis indicate that most Latino children who spoke a non-English language were doing as well as, if not better than, their White English Monolingual peers on socioemotional well-being. By fifth grade, Fluent Bilingual and Non-English-Dominant Bilingual children were surpassing every other group with the highest levels of approaches-to-learning, self-control, and interpersonal skills and the lowest levels of internalizing and externalizing behavior problems. English-Dominant Bilingual children had similar levels and trajectories of socioemotional well-being as those of White English Monolingual children. Non-English Monolingual children, however, had the lowest self-control and interpersonal skills and the highest level of internalizing problems by fifth grade, as rated by their teachers.

Introduction

Researchers and policymakers at both the national and local levels have focused much energy on children's academic performance, but less attention has been paid to another important aspect of well-being: socioemotional development. Children's socioemotional well-being, particularly during the early school years, is important in its own right and should not be overlooked by scholars. Children may have a difficult time learning to read if they have emotional and behavioral problems that distract them from academic learning (Entwisle et al., 2005, Raver, 2002, Segal, 2006), and young children who struggle with reading and learning difficulties may grow increasingly frustrated and more disruptive (Raver, 2002).

Latino children, a rapidly growing ethnic group in the United States, often do not speak English at home. However, previous studies have found that youths of second generation and beyond tend not to preserve their parents' linguistic heritage (Portes & Hao, 1998). For these reasons, this paper examines the possible connection between bilingualism and children's socioemotional trajectories during their early school years. Results from these analyses suggest that policymakers should view bilingualism as a strength that can be fostered and encouraged in their efforts to promote children's success in school. Building upon previous research on bilingualism (Portes & Hao, 1998, Portes & Hao, 2002, Portes & Hao, 2004) as well as on effective-school literature (Rutter & Maughan, 2002), this study controls for a large set of child, family, and school characteristics in order to assess the net effects of language proficiency on children's socioemotional trajectories, paying particular attention to the effects of school environments.

What does language capacity have to do with a child's social development? The early school years are a pivotal time when children are in a new, more advanced stage of cognitive development. Young students confront issues of social comparison and competition on a larger scale than they have likely experienced before (Eccles, 1999). This may be felt more acutely by English Language Learners (ELLs), who may feel more isolated and be more likely to receive “failure feedback” from their peers and teachers if they are unable to communicate well with them (Olsen, 1997). In addition, the introduction to the school environment may represent ELL children's first nonfamilial social environment as well as their first time in a different cultural environment. The negative effects of social comparison and failure feedback may have unique implications for children who are beginning to see their own cultural identities as different from those of their peers. ELL children may feel particularly discouraged and insecure around their fluent classmates. Young children who are rejected by their peers in their early years of schooling face the grave risk of lower academic achievement, a greater likelihood of grade retention and/or dropping out of school, and a greater risk of delinquency and of committing juvenile offenses in adolescence (Jimerson et al., 2000, Raver, 2002, Richman et al., 1986, Rose et al., 1989, Vitaro et al., 2001).

If language proficiency is an obstacle for ELL children, then it follows that English-only instruction could be the optimal way to improve these students' communication capacity and avoid language failure feedback. However, more than two decades of research have documented the benefits of being bilingual, including higher academic performance (e.g., Golash-Boza, 2005, Portes & Hao, 1998, Portes & Hao, 2002, Portes & Hao, 2004, Portes & McLeod, 1999, Portes & Schauffler, 1994), higher self-esteem (e.g., Portes & Hao, 2002), and stronger family cohesion (e.g., Portes & Hao, 2002, Tseng & Fuligni, 2000). Some scholars have linked bilingualism with greater cognitive flexibility and abstract thinking skills (Bialystok, 1988, Duncan & De Avilla, 1979, Rumbaut, 1995, Willig, 1985), while other researchers have examined the access that bilingual children have to positive “cultural capital” in their ethnic families and communities (Bankston & Zhou, 1995, Portes & Rumbaut, 2001, Portes & Zhou, 1993, Rumberger & Larson, 1998, Zhou & Bankston, 1994, Zhou & Bankston, 1998). These results challenge the notion that a rapid shift to monolingual English fluency is best for children's success.

Unfortunately, research has found that early generation children of immigrants tend to have stronger academic performance, better health, and more positive behavior than their native-born peers, these trends tend not to sustain through each subsequent generation. Scholars have attributed this phenomenon to the possibility that as children become more Americanized, they lose the protective effect of their parents' culture, which usually puts great emphasis on the value of education and respect for family members (Rumbaut, 1994). Moreover, with each generation, children in immigrant families become increasingly reluctant to speak the mother tongue of their parents or grandparents.

Family background, of course, plays a significant role in shaping children's experiences and developmental trajectories. This does not, however, attenuate the importance of the school in cultivating children's optimal achievement, especially ELL children, whose feelings and actions are almost certainly affected by the language feedback that they receive from their teachers and peers (Hamre & Pianta, 2001, Raver, 2002). As one example, children may not be motivated to speak their parent's language if few of their peers and teachers speak the language or if they are not given the opportunity to use their language at school. Therefore, children may not practice their family's native language despite their parents' encouragement to do so. On the other hand, the opportunity to gain English proficiency at school is vital for children who are exposed to a limited-English environment at home. Although these two issues seem contradictory, the message is the same: children's experiences and developmental trajectories, particularly those of ELL children, do not solely depend on family background but on the school environment as well.

Segmented assimilation theory postulates that the various paths taken by immigrants and their children depend not only on their family characteristics and reasons for migrating, but also on the neighborhoods in which they settle and the corresponding resources afforded to them (Portes & Zhou, 1993). For ELL students, the resources available at school supplement a family's resources to allow children to enjoy an optimal learning environment. The school can also play an important role in helping children retain their ethnic identity (i.e., by maintaining or improving proficiency in the native language) so that they can achieve selective acculturation, which, in theory, allows children to benefit from the strengths of both the U.S culture and their home culture (Portes & Hao, 2002).

In general, of course, well-run schools have much to offer students. The consensus in the effective-school literature is that students' socioemotional well-being increases when they attend schools with strong administrative leadership; adequate resources (e.g., funding); efforts made to improve partnerships between the home and school; a safe and orderly school environment; professional development activities for teachers and services for families; and classrooms organized as learning environments staffed by well-qualified teachers and aides (Bennett et al., 2005, Borman & Overman, 2004, Griffith, 2002, Rutter & Maughan, 2002). For example, studies have observed that at schools in which the principal provides strong leadership including a strategic vision, staff participation with shared vision and goals, and an effective home–school partnership, students have better well-being in general and stronger academic performance in particular (Bennett et al., 2005, Borman & Overman, 2004, Rutter & Maughan, 2002). Further, prior research indicates that a safe and orderly school atmosphere reaffirms the types of positive social behaviors that resilient children often possess (Smith & Carlson, 1997, Rutter & Maughan, 2002). Providing age-appropriate, clearly conveyed expectations and making concerted efforts to communicate with parents about children's learning process and curriculum are important features of fostering school effectiveness (Rutter & Maughan, 2002).

Unfortunately, studies have found that schools serving low-income, minority, immigrant, and ELL children can jeopardize students' well-being by failing to provide a supportive school climate—mainly by institutionalizing low academic expectations, having inadequate educational resources, and suffering from high levels of teacher and administrative turnover (Borman & Overman, 2004, Griffith, 2002, Griffith, 2003, Suárez-Orozco & Suárez-Orozco, 2001, Valencia, 2000). Students' socioemotional well-being might also suffer in schools where teachers have fewer positive (or more negative) interactions with students from low-income families or in schools with a high concentration of minority or poverty-stricken students (Hamre & Pianta, 2001, Pianta et al., 2002, Stuhlman & Pianta, 2004). This possibility is alarming because the teacher–child relationship is a salient marker of the socioemotional processes involved in academic performance (Hamre & Pianta, 2001). Further, children who are vulnerable in some way, such as having limited English, are likely to be influenced more than other children by their experiences in school contexts (Entwisle & Alexander, 1998).

In addition to the factors that are important to foster effective schools, the type of language instruction provided by the schools is important in helping ELL students improve their overall English proficiency and communicate better with teachers and peers. The communication skills of ELL children may, in turn, have profound implications for their adaptation to the school environment and thus their socioemotional well-being. In an effort to improve the English-language abilities of children of immigrants, schools have implemented both English-only and bilingual instruction approaches. Recent research suggests that programs combining both the child's native language and English need to be implemented for at least 6 to 8 years for their long-term beneficial effects on ELL students' school performance to become apparent (Collier & Thomas, 2004). In practice, transitional or remedial language programs designed to move ELL students into English-only instruction are more prevalent (Zehler et al., 2003). The 2001 No Child Left Behind Act (NCLB), which holds public schools accountable for the academic performance of ELL students, has only amplified this trend. This act requires ELL students to take standardized tests in English within 3 years of entering the school system. English-only services, however, run the risk of shifting ELL children to English monolingualism at the expense of developing their native language abilities. In contrast, bilingual instruction ideally supports the simultaneous development of both English and the native language (August & Hakuta, 1997, Cziko, 1992, Lucas et al., 1997, Olsen, 1997). To the extent that bilingualism is related to children's academic outcomes, these programs could have very different influences on children's development.

More importantly, whether a school provides an environment that supports optimal socioemotional development largely depends upon the continuity and stability of that environment (Pianta & Walsh, 1998). For example, a well-designed bilingual program may not fully promote children's well-being if students only receive the instruction for a short period of time. Likewise, a high level of teacher turnover may also reduce the benefits of a well-designed curriculum because of the resulting discontinuity and interruptions to children's learning processes. Continuity and stability play a vital role in children's well-being and are also, not surprisingly, important factors in creating an integrated and learner-friendly school environment. Other important inputs for this type of environment include qualified teachers, sound leadership, and a well-designed curriculum (August & Shanahan, 2008, Lucas et al., 1997, Rutter & Maughan, 2002).

Taken together, students' socioemotional development is fostered in an ecological context, with surrounding environments–like schools–shaping how and what children learn and experience every day. We also know that the early school years constitute a critical period for children's later socioemotional well-being (Entwisle & Hayduck, 1988). Previous research on adolescent immigrants has also shown that being bilingual has benefits for academic achievement, self-worth, and family solidarity. What we do not yet know is how bilingualism factors into children's socioemotional development during their early school years and how the school environment influences these developmental processes. For these reasons it is vital to understand how schools shape the early socioemotional trajectories and later well-being of young children, as well as how children's socioemotional well-being might differ given their language proficiency in the early school years. This study fills this knowledge gap.

Section snippets

Data

The ECLS-K, collected by the U.S. Department of Education's National Center for Educational Statistics, consists of longitudinal data on a nationally representative cohort of children who entered kindergarten in the fall of 1998. Children were drawn randomly via a multistage probability design from a nationally representative sample of roughly 1000 U.S. public and private schools. The ECLS-K sample began with 21,260 children in about 800 public and 200 private schools at entry to kindergarten

Social and emotional well-being

The socioemotional assessment draws teacher-reported data from the Social Rating Scale (Gresham & Elliott, 1990) and focuses on the skills and behaviors that contribute to social competence. These skills include social skills (e.g., cooperation, assertion, responsibility, and self-control) and problem behaviors (e.g., impulsive reactions, verbal and physical aggression) as measured through five scales. Approaches-to-learning, self-control, and interpersonal skills were rated on a scale of 1 (

Methods

Three-level growth-curve modeling was used to estimate the associations between language status and children's socioemotional trajectories. Analyses were estimated with Level 1 as time (i.e., within-individual effects), Level 2 as individuals (i.e., between-individual and within-school effects), and Level 3 as schools (i.e., between-school effects). Using longitudinal data from five assessment points (from K to 5), children's developmental trajectories (growth/decay curves) were estimated to

Results

The raw data, which is not shown for brevity's sake, indicate that Latino children, regardless of generation status, tended to fare more poorly than White children, both according to a number of socio-demographic variables (e.g., family socioeconomic status) and in their parents' involvement in their schooling. Still, Latino children tended to have parents who held higher educational expectations for their children. Regarding school characteristics, Latino children were more likely than White

Discussion and conclusion

By fifth grade, most of the Latino children in the study sample who spoke a non-English language were doing as well as, if not better than, their White English Monolingual peers on various measures of socioemotional well-being. Fluent Bilingual and Non-English-Dominant Bilingual children were surpassing every other group on all five socioemotional outcomes, with the highest levels of approaches-to-learning, self-control, and interpersonal skills and the lowest levels of internalizing and

Acknowledgments

The author gratefully acknowledges support from the Foundation for Child Development PK-3 Initiatives, as well as the partial support from the National Taiwan University Social Policy Research Center.

References (61)

  • G.D. Borman et al.

    Academic resilience in mathematics among poor and minority students

    The Elementary School Journal

    (2004)
  • V.P. Collier et al.

    The astounding effectiveness of dual language education for all

    NABE Journal of Research and Practice

    (2004)
  • G.Q. Conchas

    Structuring failure and success: Understanding the variability in Latino school engagement

    Harvard Educational Review

    (2001)
  • G.A. Cziko

    The evaluation of bilingual education: From necessity and probability to possibility

    Educational Researcher

    (1992)
  • S.E. Duncan et al.

    PreLAS 2000 examiner's manual, English forms C and D

    (2000)
  • S.E. Duncan et al.

    Bilingualism and cognition: Some recent findings

    NABE Journal

    (1979)
  • J.S. Eccles

    The development of children ages 6–14

    The Future of Children

    (1999)
  • D.R. Entwisle et al.

    Facilitating the transition to first grade: The nature of transition and research on factors affecting it

    The Elementary School Journal

    (1998)
  • D.R. Entwisle et al.

    Lasting effects of elementary school

    Sociology of Education

    (1988)
  • D.R. Entwisle et al.

    First grade and educational attainment by age 22: A new story

    American Journal of Sociology

    (2005)
  • FCD Policy Brief
  • A.J. Fuligni

    The academic achievement of adolescents from immigrant families: The roles of family background, attitudes, and behavior

    Child Development

    (1997)
  • A.J. Fuligni

    Parental authority, adolescent autonomy, and parent–adolescent relationships: A study of adolescents from Mexican, Chinese, Filipino, and European backgrounds

    Developmental Psychology

    (1998)
  • T. Golash-Boza

    Assessing the advantages of bilingualism for the children of immigrants

    International Migration Review

    (2005)
  • F.M. Gresham et al.

    The social skills rating system

    (1990)
  • J. Griffith

    A multilevel analysis of the relation of school learning and social environments to minority achievement in public elementary schools

    The Elementary School Journal

    (2002)
  • J. Griffith

    Schools as organizational models: Implications for examining school effectiveness

    The Elementary School Journal

    (2003)
  • B.K. Hamre et al.

    Early teacher–child relationships and the trajectories of children's school outcomes through eighth grades

    Child Development

    (2001)
  • J. Hox

    Multilevel analysis: Techniques and applications

    (2002)
  • T. Lucas et al.

    Promoting the success of Latino language-minority students: An exploratory study of six high schools

  • Cited by (93)

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text