Elsevier

Child Abuse & Neglect

Volume 40, February 2015, Pages 124-131
Child Abuse & Neglect

Handgrip force of maltreating mothers in reaction to infant signals

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2014.03.006Get rights and content

Abstract

Handgrip force responses to infant signals were examined in a sample of 43 maltreating and 40 non-maltreating mothers. During a standardized handgrip paradigm, mothers were asked to squeeze a handgrip dynamometer at maximal and at half of their maximal handgrip strength while listening to infant crying and laughter sounds. Maltreating mothers used excessive force more often while listening to infant crying and laughter than non-maltreating mothers. Of the maltreating mothers, only neglectful mothers (n = 20) tended to use excessive force more often during crying than non-maltreating mothers. Participants did not rate the sounds differently, indicating that maltreating mothers cannot be differentiated from non-maltreating mothers based on their perception of infant signals, but show different behavioral responses to the signals. Results imply that, in response to infant signals (i.e., crying or laughing), maltreating mothers may be insufficiently able to regulate the exertion of physical force.

Section snippets

Sample

A group of 43 maltreating and 40 non-maltreating mothers were recruited at a mental health facility. The maltreating mothers received family treatment either as outpatients or as inpatients, and were informed about the study before the start of their treatment. The non-maltreating mothers were recruited from another unit within the same mental health facility, specialized in child developmental or learning problems. At least one of their children was receiving treatment. Eligible mothers in

Perception of cry and laughter sounds

Independent samples t-tests were conducted to examine group differences on the overall perception scores. No significant differences for perception of laughter or crying were found for current maltreatment status (ps > .37), nor for experienced childhood maltreatment (ps > .81). There was no significant main effect of type of current maltreatment (abuse or neglect) on the overall perception scores (ps > .50). Thus, the two groups of participants did not rate the sounds differently. In general, crying

Discussion

As hypothesized, maltreating mothers used excessive force significantly more often than did non-maltreating mothers. Maltreating mothers did not rate the sounds as more negative than non-maltreating mothers. This indicates that maltreating mothers cannot be differentiated from non-maltreating mothers based on their perception of the child signals. However, they can be differentiated from non-maltreating mothers based on their ability to modulate behavioral responses. Distinguishing between the

References (50)

  • J.A. Aragona et al.

    Neglected children: Mothers’ report of child behavior problems and observed verbal behavior

    Child Development

    (1981)
  • A. Arteche et al.

    The effects of postnatal maternal depression and anxiety on the processing of infant faces

    Journal of Affective Disorders

    (2011)
  • R. Baer

    Mindfulness training as a clinical intervention: A conceptual and empirical review

    Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice

    (2003)
  • M.J. Bakermans-Kranenburg et al.

    Oxytocin decreases handgrip force in reaction to infant crying in females without harsh parenting experiences

    Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience

    (2012)
  • D. Barnett et al.

    Defining child maltreatment: The interface between policy and research

  • L.J. Berlin et al.

    Examining pregnant women's hostile attributions about infants as a predictor of offspring maltreatment

    JAMA Pediatrics

    (2013)
  • D.P. Bernstein et al.

    Validity of the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire in an adolescent psychiatric population

    Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry

    (1997)
  • D.P. Bernstein et al.

    Childhood Trauma Questionnaire: A retrospective self-report manual

    (1998)
  • D.P. Bernstein et al.

    Initial reliability and validity of a new retrospective measure of child abuse and neglect

    American Journal of Psychiatry

    (1994)
  • D.P. Bernstein et al.

    Development and validation of a brief screening version of the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire

    Child Abuse & Neglect

    (2003)
  • D.M. Bousha et al.

    Mother-child interactional style in abuse, neglect, and control groups: Naturalistic observations in the home

    Journal of Abnormal Psychology

    (1984)
  • J. Bowlby
    (1969/1982)
  • D.B. Bugental et al.

    In charge but not in control: The management of teaching relationships by adults with low perceived power

    Developmental Psychology

    (1999)
  • L.A. Camras et al.

    Recognition and posing of emotional expressions by abuse children and their mothers

    Developmental Psychology

    (1988)
  • D. Cicchetti et al.

    The differential impacts of early physical and sexual abuse and internalizing problems on daytime cortisol rhythm in school-aged children

    Child Development

    (2010)
  • D. Cicchetti et al.

    Maternal maltreatment classification interview. Rochester, NY: Unpublished measure

    (2003)
  • J.L. Crouch et al.

    The Word Game: An innovative strategy for assessing implicit processes in parents at risk for child physical abuse

    Child Abuse & Neglect

    (2012)
  • J.L. Crouch et al.

    Parental responses to infant crying: The influence of child physical abuse risk and hostile priming

    Child Abuse and Neglect

    (2008)
  • M.R. Dadds et al.

    Attributions, affect, and behavior in abuse-risk mothers: A laboratory study

    Child Abuse & Neglect

    (2003)
  • M. Dong et al.

    The interrelatedness of multiple forms of childhood abuse, neglect, and household dysfunction

    Child Abuse & Neglect

    (2004)
  • H. Dubowitz et al.

    A conceptual definition of child neglect

    Criminal Justice and Behavior

    (1993)
  • B. Egeland et al.

    Breaking the cycle of abuse

    Child development

    (1988)
  • S. Euser et al.

    The prevalence of child maltreatment in the Netherlands across a 5-year period

    Child Abuse & Neglect

    (2013)
  • M.M. Farc et al.

    Hostility ratings by parents at risk for child abuse: Impact of chronic and temporary schema activation

    Child Abuse & Neglect

    (2008)
  • A.M. Frodi et al.

    Child abusers’ responses to infant smiles and cries

    Child Development

    (1980)
  • Cited by (20)

    • Childhood maltreatment moderates the relationship between emotion recognition and maternal sensitive behaviors

      2020, Child Abuse and Neglect
      Citation Excerpt :

      It appears that this responsiveness to child distress is particularly apparent among mothers who experienced childhood maltreatment (Buisman et al., 2018) or more coercive discipline (Schoenmaker et al., 2015), and those with an insecure style (Ablow et al., 2013; Riem, Bakermans-Kranenburg, van IJzendoorn, Out, & Rombouts, 2012). Moreover, mothers who show greater behavioral reactions to crying children tend to show less sensitive behaviors during mother-child interactions (Buisman et al., 2018; Compier-de Block et al., 2015). Our results indicate that visual emotional signals are perceived similarly to auditory ones.

    • Maternal childhood adversity, OXTR genotype and cognitive load impact on perceptual and behavioral responses to infant crying

      2019, Psychoneuroendocrinology
      Citation Excerpt :

      The sound stimuli were presented through loudspeakers, with the crying infant and the tone sound being presented for two minutes x four times and with half being under a high cognitive load condition and the other half being under a low cognitive load condition. As in previous studies (Buisman et al., 2018; Compier-de Block et al., 2015), the grip strength modulation was calculated by dividing the half-strength squeeze intensity by the full-strength squeeze intensity; therefore, scores over 0.50 indicated excessive force for the half-strength squeeze attempt. The higher the score of handgrip force are, the lesser the participants could regulate their emotion.

    • The past is present: The role of maltreatment history in perceptual, behavioral and autonomic responses to infant emotional signals

      2018, Child Abuse and Neglect
      Citation Excerpt :

      Test–retest reliability for handgrip strength measurement has been shown to be adequate (0.91 for men and 0.94 for women across a 10-week period; Reddon, Stefanyk, Gill, & Renney, 1985). Following previous studies (e.g., Compier-de Block et al., 2015) grip strength modulation was calculated by dividing the half-strength squeeze intensity by the full-strength squeeze intensity, so that scores of over 0.50 indicated excessive force on the half-strength squeeze attempt. Scores were then multiplied by 10 to scale up the variance.

    View all citing articles on Scopus

    This study was supported by Yulius Mental Health Clinic and by the Netherlands Organisation for Scientific Research (MJBK: VICI; MHvIJ: SPINOZA). We are grateful to Maureen Meekel for her technical assistance.

    View full text