Elsevier

Child Abuse & Neglect

Volume 36, Issues 7–8, July–August 2012, Pages 564-571
Child Abuse & Neglect

Reading between the lines: Implicit assessment of the association of parental attributions and empathy with abuse risk

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2012.05.004Get rights and content

Abstract

Objective

Researchers in the child maltreatment field have traditionally relied on explicit self-reports to study factors that may exacerbate physical child abuse risk. The current investigation evaluated an implicit analog task utilizing eye tracking technology to assess both parental attributions of child misbehavior and empathy.

Method

Based on the observation that readers experience comprehension difficulty when encountering passages inconsistent with their beliefs, an eye tracker gauged the extent of difficulty parents experienced reading vignettes that inappropriately characterized a child as culpable for misbehavior and that presented a non-empathic child interaction.

Results

Results suggest self-reports of attributions and empathy are related to both child abuse potential and discipline intentions; however, the eye tracking analog for empathy correlated with abuse potential but not punishment decisions whereas the analog for attributions correlated with punishment decisions but not abuse potential.

Conclusions

Such contrasts between self-report and analog assessment underscore the need for continued research studying theorized abuse risk constructs using alternative approaches to better identify the important risk markers associated with elevated child abuse risk and to minimize methodological overlap.

Introduction

Researchers have invested considerable energy attempting to predict the likelihood a parent will engage in abuse, a concept typically referred to as child abuse potential (Milner, 1994). Child abuse potential has been linked with harsh parenting style and more frequent use of parent-child aggression, including abusive tactics (Rodriguez, 2010a). Not surprisingly, abusive parents typically administer excessive, harsh discipline (Veltkamp & Miller, 1994). Greater confidence regarding which factors truly elevate child abuse risk is thereby critical for advancing prevention and intervention efforts. The current study presents a preliminary investigation into a novel approach to studying factors previously theorized to predict physical child abuse potential.

One model of cognitive processes that theoretically lead a parent to engage in physical abuse is Social Information Processing (SIP) theory, delineated by Milner (2000). SIP theory postulates that pre-existing cognitive schema (e.g., a parent's child- and discipline-related beliefs) exist prior to processing information from a new interaction. Then, when faced with a discipline situation, 4 stages begin. The parent must first accurately attend to and perceive the situation in Stage 1, then in Stage 2, the parent develops expectations and interpretations of the situation. Problems with accurate perceptions followed by biases in interpretation may be compounded by the parent's difficulty integrating mitigating information that could account for the child's behavior or difficulty considering alternative responses in Stage 3. In Stage 4, the parent who selects physical discipline encounters difficulty monitoring their implementation of discipline that could culminate in abuse. Considerable research supports individual elements of stages (e.g., Milner, 2000, Rodriguez, 2010b) as well as multiple components from different stages (e.g., De Paúl et al., 2006, Rodriguez and Richardson, 2007).

The present study examined 2 factors previously proposed in the SIP model, parental attributions (a Stage 2 process) and empathy (a Stage 3 process). Attributions that a child is misbehaving intentionally can affect a mother's decision to implement physical discipline (Ateah & Durrant, 2005). Abuse-risk mothers make negative dispositional attributions for children's negative behaviors yet attribute positive behaviors to external causes (Dadds, Mullins, McAllister, & Atkinson, 2003). Indeed, physically abusive mothers interpreted child misbehavior as intentional annoyance significantly more often than a matched sample of non-abusive mothers (Haskett, Scott, Willoughby, Ahern, & Nears, 2006). Collectively, research generally supports that parental attributions of children's behavior represent a potentially important component in increased abuse risk, although others have suggested that higher abuse risk parents do not demonstrate more negative attitudes toward children (Risser, Skowronski, & Crouch, 2011) and questions have arisen regarding the optimal measurement of attributions (Milner, 2003).

Negative attributions can be potentially attenuated by integrating mitigating information in the third SIP stage, such as a parent's empathic ability that could inform of a situation-specific context that lessens a child's perceived responsibility (Milner, 2000). An inability to experience empathy has been previously linked to physically abusive parenting (Feshbach, 1989, Milner and Dopke, 1997). Low empathic perspective-taking ability has been associated with greater child abuse potential in community (Rodriguez & Richardson, 2007) and at-risk samples (McElroy and Rodriguez, 2008). Empathy and attribution of child intent were specifically targeted for the current investigation because both suffer from one of the obstacles in confidently predicting their true role in elevated physical child abuse risk: our methodological reliance on measuring these concepts via self-report.

Many researchers in this field often conclude their studies citing as a limitation their dependence on eliciting information directly from participants via questionnaires, often not even anonymously. This limitation is exacerbated by designs investigating both the risk factors and the dependent variables using self-report from the same source, which raises concerns about source bias as well as questions about item or conceptual overlap between questionnaires for the predictors and the proposed outcome(s). Even in those instances when participants are assured of anonymity, such designs rely on the participants’ candor and accuracy. This strategy of obtaining self-report is considered an explicit assessment technique. Unfortunately, with more controversial constructs, explicit self-reports are more likely to lead to responses that are manipulated by participants (Fazio & Olson, 2003). Clearly this self-report design flaw is problematic for researchers in a number of fields, but this approach is particularly challenging for researchers studying child abuse risk and its related constructs (DeGarmo, Reid, & Knutson, 2006). In fact, honesty in reporting could lead to negative consequences which may be realistically appraised as threatening for parents (Bennett, Sullivan, & Lewis, 2006), inducing participants to inaccurately respond to self-report questions. Those already substantiated for abuse seem especially likely to distort their responses. Thus, respondents to questionnaires may intentionally misrepresent their true attitudes but some respondents may even unconsciously present a socially desirable image because they want to believe this positive self-perception (Fazio & Olson, 2003). Yet this field continues to rely heavily on self-report to assess risk because alternative assessment approaches are limited.

To better control biases in responding, analog tasks strive to assess a concept using implicit means (Fazio & Olson, 2003). Analog tasks are designed to assess the concept without the participant being explicitly aware of what the task is attempting to measure and/or how it is being scored. If a respondent is not overtly aware of what is being measured, they are less able to misrepresent their response. Analog tasks differ in how much automatic, unconscious processing is engaged in the task, wherein more conscious processing enables a participant to manipulate their responses (Fazio & Olson, 2003). Designing analog tasks thus demands creativity to minimize participant awareness. Another challenge for the application of analog tasks lies in the fact that some may be labor-intensive or costly. However, such assessment approaches are essential when evaluating models for abuse risk (DeGarmo et al., 2006); questions continue to arise about whether a given factor is indeed associated with abuse risk because of doubts about whether the assessment tools simply overlap in content or are manipulated by the respondent. For example, if an analog task can demonstrate an association with self-report measures of a given concept, then those utilizing the self-report measure can be more confident in the latter's use as a proxy.

Relatively few analog approaches have been considered in the child maltreatment field. Some early research investigated parents’ punitive responses under stress (Passman & Mulhern, 1977) or elicited discipline responses in real time while parents watched a video of child misbehavior (Fagot, 1992), moderately implicit approaches attempting to approximate parent discipline behavior. More recently, subliminal priming with photos has been used to study implicit parental attitudes of hostility, where abuse risk parents rated ambiguous child pictures as more hostile relative to low-risk parents (Farc, Crouch, Skowronski, & Milner, 2008), although higher abuse risk parents did not evidence more negative attitudes toward children (Risser et al., 2011). Higher abuse risk parents have greater difficulty processing either positive or negative stimuli when it is incongruent with a subliminal prime rather than simply evidencing a bias toward negative stimuli only (Crouch et al., 2010). This recent line of research highlights the need to clarify some of the implicit cognitive processing occurring in parents.

The current study evaluated an analog approach based on eye tracking technology that requires no participant response and thus would be relatively free from conscious processing. Eye trackers monitor and analyze the movement of the eye during reading (Rayner, 1998). While reading, eyes display short ballistic movements, known as saccades, or pause on sections of text, known as fixations. English is read left to right but readers occasionally evidence right-to-left eye movements, known as regressions, to review previously read material (Rayner, 1998). Reading speed is affected by the frequency and duration of fixations and regressions as well as one's overall comprehension of the material (Rayner, Chace, Slattery, & Ashby, 2006). Reading researchers have found that readers spend more time reading and rereading text inconsistent with the reader's expectations or general world knowledge (Cook and Guéraud, 2005, Cook and Myers, 2004, Rayner et al., 2006). Based on these findings, we measured parents’ eye movements as they read two types of vignettes: stories either appropriately or inappropriately represented children as culpable for an incident; stories that portrayed either an empathic or non-empathic scenario. Consider the first example in Table 1. In the culpable condition, the child is instructed not to touch the cupcakes, but the child eats them anyway. The subsequent target sentence, “The child knew their behavior was wrong….” should be easy to comprehend, and reading times should be relatively fast. However, in the non-culpable condition, where the child is instructed to get their father and does so, the subsequent target sentence should be much more difficult to comprehend. Thus, reading times for the target sentence in the non-culpable condition should be much slower than in the culpable condition. The second example in Table 1 contains a target sentence (“Fred saw Mary and laughed at the look on her face”) that either represents an empathic or non-empathic response. The target sentence should be more difficult to understand, and thus should be read more slowly, in the non-empathic condition than in the empathic condition. Based on these principles, we utilized eye tracking technology to assess reading speed, evaluating whether individuals would evidence greater difficulties reading material that inappropriately presented children as culpable or presented a non-empathic scenario.

The present study investigated whether an analog task based on the reading inconsistency paradigm could assess both parental attribution of child intentionality and empathy as it may relate to physical abuse risk and punishment intentions. Parents self-reported on their child abuse potential and punishment intentions as well as on their perception of their empathic ability and attributions of child intent. Analog scores from the eye tracking task were compared to scores on explicit measures of self-reported empathy and attributions and abuse potential and punishment intentions. Because the analog approach is entirely implicit (no participant response is required), eye tracking scores were expected to show modest correlations with explicit measures.

Specifically, parents who demonstrated greater difficulty reading non-empathic vignettes were predicted to self-report greater empathy, obtain lower child abuse potential scores, and indicate less inclination to punish children. Similarly, parents who demonstrated greater difficulty reading inappropriate implications of child culpability were expected to self-report less inclination to believe children misbehave intentionally to be annoying and would demonstrate lower abuse potential and less intention to punish misbehavior. Such a design will provide a novel approach to considering how these two theorized SIP processes, attributions and empathy, may indeed relate to elevated child abuse risk.

Section snippets

Participants

The study recruited 37 parents for a study of novel approaches to investigating parenting. However, 3 participants could not be accurately tracked to begin eye tracker recording and 8 others demonstrated poor calibration or missing/inaccurate tracking data. It is not uncommon for participant data to be dropped due to such calibration issues (Rayner, 1998) so we retained participants for whom we were confident in the quality of their eye tracking data. Those retained did not differ from the

Results

Correlations between self-reports were first considered to confirm potential associations previously identified in the literature using more conventional approaches. As seen in Table 2, self-reported attributions of child intentionality on the PCV Annoyance scale were significantly associated with greater child abuse potential scores on both the AAPI-2 Total and CAPI Abuse Scale. Negative attributions toward children (PCV Annoyance) were predictably associated with their reported greater

Discussion

The present study investigated a fully implicit analog assessment of parental attributions and empathy as they relate to physical child abuse potential. Parental attributions of child negative intentions and empathy were assessed implicitly by using eye tracking to measure parents’ processing difficulty as they read vignettes relating to child culpability or empathic responses. Findings from this study suggest that parents who self-reported greater empathy indeed slowed when reading

References (34)

  • S.J. Bavolek et al.

    Adult-Adolescent Parenting Inventory (AAPI-2): Administration and development handbook

    (2001)
  • D.S. Bennett et al.

    Relations of parental report and observation of parenting to maltreatment history

    Child Maltreatment

    (2006)
  • A.E. Cook et al.

    What have we been missing? The role of general world knowledge in discourse processing

    Discourse Processes

    (2005)
  • J. De Paúl et al.

    Impact of stress and mitigating information on evaluations, attributions, affect, disciplinary choices, and expectations of compliance in mothers at high and low risk for child physical abuse

    Journal of Interpersonal Violence

    (2006)
  • D.S. DeGarmo et al.

    Direct laboratory observations and analog measures in research definitions of child maltreatment

  • B.I. Fagot

    Assessment of coercive parent discipline

    Behavioral Assessment

    (1992)
  • R.H. Fazio et al.

    Implicit measures in social cognition research: Their meaning and use

    Annual Review of Psychology

    (2003)
  • Cited by (26)

    • Applying Socio-Emotional Information Processing theory to explain child abuse risk: Emerging patterns from the COVID-19 pandemic

      2023, Child Abuse and Neglect
      Citation Excerpt :

      Empirical research has identified that pre-existing beliefs approving of physical discipline are a precursor for PCA (Lansford et al., 2014; McCarthy et al., 2016; Rodriguez et al., 2011; Smith Slep & O'Leary, 2007). Furthermore, parents who hold negative child intent attributions (an SIP Stage 2 process) evidence greater abuse risk (Azar et al., 2013; Berlin et al., 2013; Camilo et al., 2020; Rodriguez et al., 2012, 2020). Another cognition that may relate to parents' SIP Stage 2 interpretations during parent-child conflict is rarely examined in the literature—namely, whether a parent justifies PCA use during conflict.

    • Underlying mechanisms for racial disparities in parent-child physical and psychological aggression and child abuse risk

      2021, Child Abuse and Neglect
      Citation Excerpt :

      Pre-existing schemas such as parents’ approval of using PCA as a discipline tool is a robust predictor of abuse risk and parents’ actual use of PCA (Chiocca, 2017; Lansford et al., 2014; Rodriguez, Bower Russa, & Harmon, 2011). Additionally, parents who interpret their children’s behavior with negative intent attributions evidence greater child abuse risk (Camilo, Vaz Garrido, & Calheiros, 2020; Haskett, Scott, Willoughby, Ahern, & Nears, 2006; Rodriguez, Cook, & Jedrziewski, 2012). The SIP theory for parent-child aggression emphasizes cognitive processes, although the role of emotion has been highlighted in SIP models applied to other forms of aggression (e.g., Lemerise & Arsenio, 2000).

    • Refining social-information processing theory: Predicting maternal and paternal parent-child aggression risk longitudinally

      2020, Child Abuse and Neglect
      Citation Excerpt :

      Indeed, poor frustration tolerance (McElroy & Rodriguez, 2008; Rodriguez, Russa, & Kircher, 2015; Rodriguez, Baker, Pu, & Tucker, 2017) and emotion regulation difficulties (Hien, Cohen, Caldeira, Flom, & Wasserman, 2010; Hiraoka et al., 2016) are associated with heightened child abuse risk. For Stage 2, negative attributions about children’s behavior have been observed in abusive mothers (Haskett, Scott, Willoughby, Ahern, & Nears, 2006) and in those with higher PCA risk (Azar, Okado, Stevenson, & Robinson, 2013; Berlin, Dodge, & Reznick, 2013; Rodriguez, Cook, & Jedrziewski, 2012). Also at Stage 2, at-risk parents appear to expect more compliance from children following discipline (Rodriguez et al., 2016b).

    • Predictors of change in mothers’ and fathers’ parent-child aggression risk

      2018, Child Abuse and Neglect
      Citation Excerpt :

      For example, parents’ discipline schemas may include approval of PCA as a discipline approach, wherein such approval is associated with increased PCA risk (McCarthy, Crouch, Basham, Milner, & Skowronski, 2016; Rodriguez, Bower-Russa, & Harmon, 2011). Parents who also negatively evaluate children’s behavior—ascribing negative intent to their behavior—are more likely to have been identified as abusive (Haskett, Scott, Willoughby, Ahern, & Nears, 2006), to engage in later maltreatment (Berlin, Dodge, & Reznick, 2013), and to evidence greater PCA risk (Azar, Okado, Stevenson, & Robinson, 2013; Montes, de Paúl, & Milner, 2001; Rodriguez, Cook, & Jedrziewski, 2012; Rodriguez & Tucker, 2015). Further, SIP proposes that parents need to incorporate all information in a discipline encounter, including their options for discipline.

    • Negative parental attributions mediate associations between risk factors and dysfunctional parenting: A replication and extension

      2018, Child Abuse and Neglect
      Citation Excerpt :

      The objective of the current study was to replicate the previous study using a larger sample, and to extend the findings by also including fathers, using an additional risk factor (i.e., a general child abuse risk), and including observed parenting in addition to questionnaire data. Because many studies found general abuse risk to be associated with parental attribution (e.g., Chilamkurti & Milner, 1993; De Paul et al., 2006; Irwin et al., 2014; Rodriguez, Cook, & Jedrziewski, 2012; Rodriguez & Tucker, 2015), we added this risk factor to our study. Finally, the separate mediation effects were tested in a multiple mediation model for mothers and fathers separately.

    View all citing articles on Scopus

    This study was supported in part by a grant from the University of Utah Research Committee.

    View full text